Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Aug 8;11(8):e0160595.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160595. eCollection 2016.

Male Smokers' and Non-Smokers' Response Inhibition in Go/No-Go Tasks: Effect of Three Task Parameters

Affiliations

Male Smokers' and Non-Smokers' Response Inhibition in Go/No-Go Tasks: Effect of Three Task Parameters

Xin Zhao et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Impaired response inhibition plays a major role in many addictive behaviors. However, in studies using go/no-go tasks, findings regarding the presence of response inhibition deficits in nicotine-dependent individuals are mixed. This might be due to differences between studies on a number of task parameters. Here we aimed to identify task conditions under which go/no-go task performance deficits can be observed in smokers and to characterize the nature of such deficits. Sixty-one male students (30 smokers, 31 non-smokers) performed a go/no-go task while independently manipulating three task parameters: (1) percentage no-go trials (50% or 25%), (2) stimulus presentation time (600 ms or 200 ms), and (3) nature of no-go stimuli (cigarette related or cigarette unrelated). Three measures, reaction time on go trials and percentage correct responses on go and no-go trials, served as performance indicators. Under 200-ms but not 600-ms stimulus presentation conditions, the smokers responded faster on go trials and made more errors on both go and no-go trials than the non-smokers did. These differences occurred irrespective of the percentage of no-go trials and nature of no-go stimuli. The accuracy differences disappeared after controlling for the response time differences, suggesting a strong speed-accuracy trade-off. This study contributes to unraveling the conditions under which smokers display impaired inhibition performance and helps to characterize the nature of this impairment. Under task conditions prompting fast responding, smokers are more prone to increase response speed and to make more errors than non-smokers.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Example of a trial with a cigarette-related and cigarette-unrelated picture in the go/no-go task.
Fig 2
Fig 2
Left: Mean reaction time (+ SEM) of smokers and non-smokers on go trials of the 600-ms stimulus presentation time blocks. Values are presented separately for each no-go stimulus percentage and stimulus type condition. The only significant effect was faster overall responding on the 25% no-go trials condition than the 50% condition (p < 0.001). Right: Mean (+ SEM) proportion of trials of the 600-ms stimulus presentation time blocks on which smokers and non-smokers correctly omitted responding to the no-go stimulus, separately for each no-go stimulus percentage and stimulus type condition. The only significant effect was a higher accuracy rate for the 50% no-go trials condition than the 25% condition (p < 0.001).
Fig 3
Fig 3
Left: Mean reaction time (+ SEM) of smokers and non-smokers on go trials of the 200-ms stimulus presentation time blocks. Values are presented separately for each no-go stimulus percentage and stimulus type condition. Overall, smokers responded faster than non-smokers (p < 0.001), and the 25% no-go trial condition elicited faster responding than the 50% condition (p = 0.01), especially for smokers (p < 0.001). Middle: Mean (+ SD) proportion of trials of the 200-ms stimulus presentation time blocks on which smokers and non-smokers correctly omitted responding to the no-go stimulus. Overall, non-smokers were more accurate than smokers (p = 0.001) and response accuracy was larger in the 50% no-go trial condition than the 25% condition (p < 0.001). Right: Mean (+ SD) proportion of trials of the 200-ms stimulus presentation time blocks on which smokers and non-smokers responded to the go stimulus. Overall, non-smokers were more accurate than smokers (p = 0.001) and response accuracy was larger in the 50% no-go trial condition than the 25% condition (p < 0.001), especially for the smokers (p < 0.001).

References

    1. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000; 41:49–100. 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 . - DOI - PubMed
    1. Smith JL, Mattick RP, Jamadar S, Iredale JM. Deficits in behavioural inhibition in substance abuse and addiction: a meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014; 145:1–33. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.08.009 . - DOI - PubMed
    1. Feil J, Sheppard D, Fitzgerald PB, Yücel M, Lubman DI, Bradshaw JL. Addiction, compulsive drug seeking, and the role of frontostriatal mechanisms in regulating inhibitory control. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020; 35:248–75. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.03.001 . - DOI - PubMed
    1. Billieux J, Gay P, Rochat L, Khazaal Y, Zullino D, Van der Linden M. Lack of inhibitory control predicts cigarette smoking dependence: evidence from a non-deprived sample of light to moderate smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2010; 112:164–7. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.06.006 . - DOI - PubMed
    1. Reynolds B, Patak M, Penfold RB, Shroff P, Melanko S, Duhig AM. Laboratory and self-report assessments of impulsive behaviour in adolescent daily smokers and non-smokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2007; 15:264–71. 10.1037/1064-1297.15.3.264 . - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources