Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Nov 1;116(5):2105-2113.
doi: 10.1152/jn.00222.2016. Epub 2016 Aug 10.

The destination defines the journey: an examination of the kinematics of hand-to-mouth movements

Affiliations

The destination defines the journey: an examination of the kinematics of hand-to-mouth movements

Jason W Flindall et al. J Neurophysiol. .

Abstract

Long-train electrical stimulation of the motor and premotor cortices of nonhuman primates can produce either hand-to-mouth or grasp-to-inspect movements, depending on the precise location of stimulation. Furthermore, single-neuron recording studies identify discrete neuronal populations in the inferior parietal and ventral premotor cortices that respond uniquely to either grasp-to-eat or grasp-to-place movements, despite their identical mechanistic requirements. These studies demonstrate that the macaque motor cortex is organized around producing functional, goal-oriented movements, rather than simply fulfilling muscular prerequisites of action. In humans, right-handed hand-to-mouth movements have a unique kinematic signature; smaller maximum grip apertures are produced when grasping to eat than when grasping to place identical targets. This is evidence that the motor cortex in humans is also organized around producing functional movements. However, in both macaques and humans, grasp-to-eat/hand-to-mouth movements have always been elicited using edible targets and have (necessarily) been paired with mouth movement. It is therefore unknown whether the kinematic distinction is a natural result of grasping food and/or is simply attributable to concurrent opening of the mouth while grasping. In experiment 1, we used goal-differentiated grasping tasks, directed toward edible and inedible targets, to show that the unique kinematic signature is present even with inedible targets. In experiment 2, we used the same goal-differentiated grasping tasks, either coupled with or divorced from an open-mouth movement, to show that the signature is not attributable merely to a planned opening of the mouth during the grasp. These results are discussed in relation to the role of hand-to-mouth movements in human development, independently of grasp-to-eat behavior.

Keywords: edible; grasp; grip aperture; hand; mouth.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Closed-mouth (A) and open-container conditions (B) used in experiment 2.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Grip aperture differences between conditions in experiment 1. Left, average grip aperture over time for grasps directed toward large targets for a typical participant (participant TFO). Trials have been reaction time (RT)-aligned to best show differences in maximum grip aperture (MGA) between conditions. Right, MGAs (means and SE), collapsed across target size (small, larger), in the mouth and container goal conditions for edible and inedible objects. Difference between goal conditions was significant for both target types (*P < 0.01). Avg., average.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Grip aperture differences between conditions in experiment 2. Left, average grip aperture over time during grasps directed toward large targets for a single typical participant (participant MMS). Trials have been RT-aligned to best show MGA differences between conditions. Right, MGAs, (means and SE), collapsed across size, for open- and closed-state conditions in mouth and container goal conditions. MGAs during the open-mouth condition (i.e., when participants ate the target) were significantly smaller than MGAs in both closed-mouth (*P < 0.001) and closed-container conditions (*P < 0.008). No other conditions were significantly different from one another following Bonferroni correction (P > 0.0083).

References

    1. American Academy of Pediatrics and American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists. Breastfeeding Handbook for Physicians (2nd ed.), edited by Schanler RJ, Krebs NF, and Mass SB Elk Grove, IL: American Academy of Pediatrics, 2013.
    1. Bonini L, Rozzi S, Serventi FU, Simone L, Ferrari PF, Fogassi L. Ventral premotor and inferior parietal cortices make distinct contribution to action organization and intention understanding. Cereb Cortex 20: 1372–1385, 2010. - PubMed
    1. Bonini L, Serventi FU, Bruni S, Maranesi M, Bimbi M, Simone L, Rozzi S, Ferrari PF, Fogassi L. Selectivity for grip type and action goal in macaque inferior parietal and ventral premotor grasping neurons. J Neurophysiol 108: 1607–1619, 2012. - PubMed
    1. Bootsma RJ, Marteniuk RG, MacKenzie CL, Zaal F. The speed-accuracy trade-off in manual prehension: effects of movement amplitude, object size, and object width on kinematic characteristics. Exp Brain Res 98: 535–541, 1994. - PubMed
    1. Bruni S, Giorgetti V, Fogassi L, Bonini L. Multimodal encoding of goal-directed actions in monkey ventral premotor grasping neurons. Cereb Cortex. In press. - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources