Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Oct;70(10):2094-2104.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1223704. Epub 2016 Sep 7.

Judging the animacy of words: The influence of typicality and age of acquisition in a semantic decision task

Affiliations

Judging the animacy of words: The influence of typicality and age of acquisition in a semantic decision task

Romy Räling et al. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2017 Oct.

Abstract

The age at which members of a semantic category are learned (age of acquisition), the typicality they demonstrate within their corresponding category, and the semantic domain to which they belong (living, non-living) are known to influence the speed and accuracy of lexical/semantic processing. So far, only a few studies have looked at the origin of age of acquisition and its interdependence with typicality and semantic domain within the same experimental design. Twenty adult participants performed an animacy decision task in which nouns were classified according to their semantic domain as being living or non-living. Response times were influenced by the independent main effects of each parameter: typicality, age of acquisition, semantic domain, and frequency. However, there were no interactions. The results are discussed with respect to recent models concerning the origin of age of acquisition effects.

Keywords: Age of acquisition; Animacy decision; Semantic classification task; Typicality.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Mean response times in milliseconds for the control group, depicted as a function of typicality and age of acquisition, with standard errors of means as error bars.

References

    1. Baayen R. H., Davidson D. J., & Bates D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 - DOI
    1. Barr D. J., Levy R., Scheepers C., & Tily H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Barry C., Morrison C. M., & Ellis A. W. (1997). Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures: Effects of age of acquisition, frequency, and name agreement. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 50(3), 560–585. doi:10.1080/027249897392026 - DOI
    1. Bates D., Mächler M., Bolker B., & Walker S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1). doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01 - DOI
    1. Bates D. & Sarkar D. (2007). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes.: R package version 0.9975-13. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/

LinkOut - more resources