Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Feb;43(2):167-176.
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000300. Epub 2016 Sep 12.

Modulation of task demands suggests that semantic processing interferes with the formation of episodic associations

Affiliations

Modulation of task demands suggests that semantic processing interferes with the formation of episodic associations

Nicole M Long et al. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2017 Feb.

Abstract

Although episodic and semantic memory share overlapping neural mechanisms, it remains unclear how our pre-existing semantic associations modulate the formation of new, episodic associations. When freely recalling recently studied words, people rely on both episodic and semantic associations, shown through temporal and semantic clustering of responses. We asked whether orienting participants toward semantic associations interferes with or facilitates the formation of episodic associations. We compared electroencephalographic (EEG) activity recorded during the encoding of subsequently recalled words that were either temporally or semantically clustered. Participants studied words with or without a concurrent semantic orienting task. We identified a neural signature of successful episodic association formation whereby high-frequency EEG activity (HFA, 44-100 Hz) overlying left prefrontal regions increased for subsequently temporally clustered words, but only for those words studied without a concurrent semantic orienting task. To confirm that this disruption in the formation of episodic associations was driven by increased semantic processing, we measured the neural correlates of subsequent semantic clustering. We found that HFA increased for subsequently semantically clustered words only for lists with a concurrent semantic orienting task. This dissociation suggests that increased semantic processing of studied items interferes with the neural processes that support the formation of novel episodic associations. (PsycINFO Database Record

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Methods and behavioral results
(A) Methods. During the encoding period, participants viewed words presented for 3000 ms and separated by a variable interstimulus interval. Following the last item on the list, participants recall the study items in any order. A subset of study items were semantic associates, e.g. cat and dog in this figure. Semantic relatedness was determined using Word Association Space (WAS) values (see Methods). Encoding items were divided into four conditions based on how they were recalled: temporally clustered (Ct, black) or recalled preceding or following a study neighbor, e.g. tree and lime; semantically clustered (Cs, dark grey) or recalled preceding or following a semantic associate; not clustered (NC, light grey) or recalled preceding and following non-neighboring and non-semantically related items; or not recalled (NR, white). (B) Behavioral results. Participants show a tendency to both temporally and semantically cluster their recalls. The lag contiguity analysis (first panel) shows that participants are more likely to make transitions between study neighbors, those items separated by a lag of +/− 1, than between non-neighboring study items. Likewise, the semantic contiguity analysis (second panel) shows that participants are more likely to make transitions between semantically associated items, where increased association corresponds to increased WAS values. These effects are consistent for both no-task (blue) and task (orange) lists. The third panel shows the quantification of these contiguity effects. Temporal clustering scores are reliably greater on no-task compared to task lists, whereas semantic clustering scores are reliably greater on task compared to no-task lists. Errorbars are standard error of the mean. (C) Regions of interest. We analyzed three a priori defined ROIs, left Anterior Superior (AS), left Anterior Inferior (AI) and left Posterior Inferior (PI).
Figure 2
Figure 2. Subsequent clustering effects
The top panel shows the subsequent temporal clustering effect (SCEt) and the bottom panel shows the subsequent semantic clustering effect (SCEs) for no-task (blue) and task (orange) lists. Each line shows the difference in Z-Power between subsequently clustered and subsequently recalled, but not clustered, items, for 6 frequency bands (θL, 3–4 Hz; θH, 6–8 Hz; α, 10–14 Hz; β, 16–26 Hz; γL, 28–42 Hz; γH, 44–100 Hz). Z-Power is averaged across the encoding interval (0 – 3000 ms). Errorbars are standard error of the mean.
Figure 3
Figure 3. HFA as a function of clustering and list type
Subsequent temporal and semantic clustering effects (SCEt and SCEs) separately for no-task (blue) and task (orange) lists. Each bar shows the difference in high frequency activity between subsequently clustered (temporal or semantic) and subsequently recalled, but not clustered words. There is a reliable interaction in AS and AI such that the SCEt is specific to no-task lists and the SCEs is specific to task lists. Error bars are standard errors of the mean.
Figure 4
Figure 4. Subsequent memory effect for semantically isolated items
Semantically isolated items are those items which do not have a strong semantic study associate (WAS > .4). The figure shows HFA for semantically isolated items that are subsequently recalled (recalled, black) or subsequently not recalled (not recalled, white), separately for no-task and task lists. There is a reliable interaction in AS such that HFA is increased for not recalled relative to recalled items specifically for task lists. Error bars are standard errors of the mean.

References

    1. Badre D, Wagner A. Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(13):2883–2901. - PubMed
    1. Ball T, Schulze-Bonhage A, Aertsen A, Mehring C. Differential representation of arm movement direction in relation to cortical anatomy and function. Journal of neural engineering. 2009;6(1):016006. - PubMed
    1. Bedny M, McGill M, Thompson-Schill SL. Semantic adaptation and competition during word comprehension. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18(11):2574–2585. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Binder JR, Desai RH. The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15(11):527–536. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Blumenfeld R, Ranganath C. Prefrontal cortex and long-term memory encoding: An integrative review of findings from neuropsychology and neuroimaging. The Neuroscientist. 2007;13(3):280. - PubMed

Publication types