Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Nov 4:10:536.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00536. eCollection 2016.

Berlyne Revisited: Evidence for the Multifaceted Nature of Hedonic Tone in the Appreciation of Paintings and Music

Affiliations

Berlyne Revisited: Evidence for the Multifaceted Nature of Hedonic Tone in the Appreciation of Paintings and Music

Manuela M Marin et al. Front Hum Neurosci. .

Abstract

In his seminal book on esthetics, Berlyne (1971) posited an inverted-U relationship between complexity and hedonic tone in arts appreciation, however, converging evidence for his theory is still missing. The disregard of the multidimensionality of complexity may explain some of the divergent results. Here, we argue that definitions of hedonic tone are manifold and systematically examined whether the nature of the relationship between complexity and hedonic tone is determined by the specific measure of hedonic tone. In Experiment 1, we studied three picture categories with similar affective and semantic contents: 96 affective environmental scenes, which were also converted into 96 cartoons, and 96 representational paintings. Complexity varied along the dimension of elements. In a between-subjects design, each stimulus was presented for 5 s to 206 female participants. Subjective ratings of hedonic tone (either beauty, pleasantness or liking), arousal, complexity and familiarity were collected in three conditions per stimulus set. Complexity and arousal were positively associated in all conditions, with the strongest association observed for paintings. For environmental scenes and cartoons, there was no significant association between complexity and hedonic tone, and the three measures of hedonic tone were highly correlated (all rs > 0.85). As predicted, in paintings the measures of hedonic tone were less strongly correlated (all rs > 0.73), and when controlling for familiarity, the association with complexity was significantly positive for beauty (rs = 0.26), weakly negative for pleasantness (rs = -0.16) and not present for liking. Experiment 2 followed a similar approach and 77 female participants, all non-musicians, rated 92 musical excerpts (15 s) in three conditions of hedonic tone (either beauty, pleasantness or liking). Results indicated a strong relationship between complexity and arousal (all rs > 0.85). When controlling for familiarity effects, the relationship between complexity and beauty followed an inverted-U curve, whereas the relationship between complexity and pleasantness was negative (rs = -0.26) and the one between complexity and liking positive (rs = 0.29). We relate our results to Berlyne's theory and the latest findings in neuroaesthetics, proposing that future studies need to acknowledge the multifaceted nature of hedonic tone in esthetic experiences of artforms.

Keywords: beauty; complexity; esthetic experience; liking; neuroaesthetics; pleasantness; visual arts.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Illustration of stimulus categories employed in Experiment 1. (A) Photograph of a landscape (Manuela Marin), (B) photograph converted into a cartoon-like picture, and (C) representational painting by Paul Cézanne, The bay of Marseille, seen from L’Estaque (1885). Note that pictures (A,B) were not used in the actual experiment but are depicted for illustrative purposes only.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
The relationships between complexity and the three measures of hedonic tone for 96 IAPS pictures: (A) beauty, (B) pleasantness, and (C) liking.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
The relationships between complexity and the three measures of hedonic tone for 96 cartoons based on IAPS pictures: (A) beauty, (B) pleasantness, and (C) liking.
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
The relationships between complexity and the three measures of hedonic tone for 94 representational paintings: (A) beauty, (B) pleasantness, and (C) liking.
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
The relationships between complexity and the three measures of hedonic tone for 19th-century piano solo music: (A) beauty, (B) pleasantness, and (C) liking.

References

    1. Aldhafeeri F. M., Mackenzie I., Kay T., Alghamdi J., Sluming V. (2012). Regional brain responses to pleasant and unpleasant IAPS pictures: different networks. Neurosci. Lett. 512 94–98. 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.01.064 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Augustin M. D., Carbon C.-C., Wagemans J. (2012a). Artful terms: a study on aesthetic word usage for visual art versus film and music. i-Perception 3 319–337. 10.1068/i0511aap - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Augustin M. D., Wagemans J., Carbon C.-C. (2012b). All is beautiful? Generality vs. specificity of word usage in visual aesthetics. Acta Psychol. 139 187–201. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.10.004 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Berlyne D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and Psychobiology. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
    1. Berlyne D. E. (1974a). Studies in the New Experimental Aesthetics: Steps Toward an Objective Psychology of Aesthetic Appreciation. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.