Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2016 Jul-Dec;41(5-8):324-341.
doi: 10.1080/87565641.2016.1256403. Epub 2017 Jan 27.

The Multisensory Nature of Verbal Discourse in Parent-Toddler Interactions

Affiliations

The Multisensory Nature of Verbal Discourse in Parent-Toddler Interactions

Sumarga H Suanda et al. Dev Neuropsychol. 2016 Jul-Dec.

Abstract

Toddlers learn object names in sensory rich contexts. Many argue that this multisensory experience facilitates learning. Here, we examine how toddlers' multisensory experience is linked to another aspect of their experience associated with better learning: the temporally extended nature of verbal discourse. We observed parent-toddler dyads as they played with, and as parents talked about, a set of objects. Analyses revealed links between the multisensory and extended nature of speech, highlighting inter-connections and redundancies in the environment. We discuss the implications of these results for our understanding of early discourse, multisensory communication, and how the learning environment shapes language development.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Example image frames from toddler egocentric cameras found to be associated with object name learning (see Pereira et al., 2014).
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
The experimental set-up: Toddlers equipped with head cameras and their parents played with mono-colored objects in a laboratory room; play sessions were recorded from multiple angles.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Illustration of the automated processing of a single head camera image frame (A–C). Sampling of objects that were classified as visually dominant using the 5% field of view threshold (D).
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Representative time series of parents’ referential utterances (top row) and the nonverbal events (bottom three rows); colors reflect the target object of speech and nonverbal event.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Example time series of parents’ utterances to different objects over the course of a trial. Adjacent utterances to the same object were merged into episodes of discourse.
Figure 6.
Figure 6.
Mean proportion of utterances within extended discourse episodes that overlapped with each nonverbal property (A–C), and that were classified as at least bimodal (D) or multimodal (E).
Figure 7.
Figure 7.
Hypothetical data (top row) and predictions (bottom row) for two hypotheses on the synchronous nature of the multisensory coupling in parents’ speech. Highly synchronous coupling would lead to greater overlap with nonverbal events in speech versus nonspeech segments; less synchronous coupling would lead to greater parity in overlap with nonverbal events between speech and nonspeech segments.
Figure 8.
Figure 8.
Illustration of the relevant windows of analysis for speech and nonspeech segments (A). Mean proportion of speech and nonspeech segments that overlapped with the three nonverbal properties (B–D).
Figure 9.
Figure 9.
Example time series of discourse episodes depicting the coupling between verbal and each nonverbal property.
Figure 10.
Figure 10.
(A) Examples of relevant moments for analysis for utterances in extended and brief episodes; (B–D) Mean proportion of utterances that contained some overlap with the three nonverbal properties.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adamson LB, & Bakeman R (2006). Development of displaced speech in early mother-child conversations. Child Development, 77, 186–200. doi:10.1111/cdev.2006.77.issue-1 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Axelsson EL, Churchley K, & Horst JS (2012). The right thing at the right time: Why ostensive naming facilitates word learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 88. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00088 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bakeman R, & Gottman JM (1997). Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential analysis. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
    1. Baldwin DA, & Markman EM (1989). Establishing word-object relations: A first step. Child Development, 60, 381–398. - PubMed
    1. Bard EG, & Anderson AH (1983). The unintelligibility of speech to children. Journal of Child Language, 10, 265–292. doi:10.1017/S0305000900007777 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources