Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Jan 18:10:688.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00688. eCollection 2016.

Does Combined Physical and Cognitive Training Improve Dual-Task Balance and Gait Outcomes in Sedentary Older Adults?

Affiliations

Does Combined Physical and Cognitive Training Improve Dual-Task Balance and Gait Outcomes in Sedentary Older Adults?

Sarah A Fraser et al. Front Hum Neurosci. .

Abstract

Everyday activities like walking and talking can put an older adult at risk for a fall if they have difficulty dividing their attention between motor and cognitive tasks. Training studies have demonstrated that both cognitive and physical training regimens can improve motor and cognitive task performance. Few studies have examined the benefits of combined training (cognitive and physical) and whether or not this type of combined training would transfer to walking or balancing dual-tasks. This study examines the dual-task benefits of combined training in a sample of sedentary older adults. Seventy-two older adults (≥60 years) were randomly assigned to one of four training groups: Aerobic + Cognitive training (CT), Aerobic + Computer lessons (CL), Stretch + CT and Stretch + CL. It was expected that the Aerobic + CT group would demonstrate the largest benefits and that the active placebo control (Stretch + CL) would show the least benefits after training. Walking and standing balance were paired with an auditory n-back with two levels of difficulty (0- and 1-back). Dual-task walking and balance were assessed with: walk speed (m/s), cognitive accuracy (% correct) and several mediolateral sway measures for pre- to post-test improvements. All groups demonstrated improvements in walk speed from pre- (M = 1.33 m/s) to post-test (M = 1.42 m/s, p < 0.001) and in accuracy from pre- (M = 97.57%) to post-test (M = 98.57%, p = 0.005).They also increased their walk speed in the more difficult 1-back (M = 1.38 m/s) in comparison to the 0-back (M = 1.36 m/s, p < 0.001) but reduced their accuracy in the 1-back (M = 96.39%) in comparison to the 0-back (M = 99.92%, p < 0.001). Three out of the five mediolateral sway variables (Peak, SD, RMS) demonstrated significant reductions in sway from pre to post test (p-values < 0.05). With the exception of a group difference between Aerobic + CT and Stretch + CT in accuracy, there were no significant group differences after training. Results suggest that there can be dual-task benefits from training but that in this sedentary sample Aerobic + CT training was not more beneficial than other types of combined training.

Keywords: combined training (physical and cognitive dual-task training); dual-task balance; dual-task gait; transfer effects.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
An example of the stimuli presented and vocal response expected from the participant for the 0-back and 1-back conditions. Note: In the 0-back condition the participant hears a number “9” and immediately responds “9”. In the 1-back condition the participant hears the first stimuli “9” and has to keep it in mind until they hear the second stimuli “1” and at this point they respond with the first item they heard “9”, then they have to keep in mind the “1” until presented with the next stimuli, and so on.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean pre- and post-test values for single and dual-task walk speed for both difficulty levels. Note: Error bars = standard error of the mean.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean pre- and post-test values for single and dual-task accuracy for both difficulty levels. Note: Error bars = standard error of the mean.

References

    1. van Abbema R., De Greef M., Crajé C., Krijnen W., Hobbelen H., Van Der Schans C. (2015). What type, or combination of exercise can improve preferred gait speed in older adults? A meta-analysis. BMC Geriatr. 15:72. 10.1186/s12877-015-0061-9 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Allali G., Assal F., Kressig R. W., Dubost V., Herrmann F. R., Beauchet O. (2008). Impact of impaired executive function on gait stability. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 26, 364–369. 10.1159/000162358 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Al-Yahya E., Dawes H., Smith L., Dennis A., Howells K., Cockburn J. (2011). Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 715–728. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.008 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bherer L. (2015). Cognitive plasticity in older adults: effects of cognitive training and physical exercise. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1337, 1–6. 10.1111/nyas.12682 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bherer L., Erickson K. I., Liu-Ambrose T. (2013). A review of the effects of physical activity and exercise on cognitive and brain functions in older adults. J. Aging Res. 2013:657508. 10.1155/2013/657508 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources