Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Feb 21;12(2):e0172650.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0172650. eCollection 2017.

Poor replication validity of biomedical association studies reported by newspapers

Affiliations

Poor replication validity of biomedical association studies reported by newspapers

Estelle Dumas-Mallet et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Objective: To investigate the replication validity of biomedical association studies covered by newspapers.

Methods: We used a database of 4723 primary studies included in 306 meta-analysis articles. These studies associated a risk factor with a disease in three biomedical domains, psychiatry, neurology and four somatic diseases. They were classified into a lifestyle category (e.g. smoking) and a non-lifestyle category (e.g. genetic risk). Using the database Dow Jones Factiva, we investigated the newspaper coverage of each study. Their replication validity was assessed using a comparison with their corresponding meta-analyses.

Results: Among the 5029 articles of our database, 156 primary studies (of which 63 were lifestyle studies) and 5 meta-analysis articles were reported in 1561 newspaper articles. The percentage of covered studies and the number of newspaper articles per study strongly increased with the impact factor of the journal that published each scientific study. Newspapers almost equally covered initial (5/39 12.8%) and subsequent (58/600 9.7%) lifestyle studies. In contrast, initial non-lifestyle studies were covered more often (48/366 13.1%) than subsequent ones (45/3718 1.2%). Newspapers never covered initial studies reporting null findings and rarely reported subsequent null observations. Only 48.7% of the 156 studies reported by newspapers were confirmed by the corresponding meta-analyses. Initial non-lifestyle studies were less often confirmed (16/48) than subsequent ones (29/45) and than lifestyle studies (31/63). Psychiatric studies covered by newspapers were less often confirmed (10/38) than the neurological (26/41) or somatic (40/77) ones. This is correlated to an even larger coverage of initial studies in psychiatry. Whereas 234 newspaper articles covered the 35 initial studies that were later disconfirmed, only four press articles covered a subsequent null finding and mentioned the refutation of an initial claim.

Conclusion: Journalists preferentially cover initial findings although they are often contradicted by meta-analyses and rarely inform the public when they are disconfirmed.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Preferential coverage of initial findings and influence of the impact factor (IF).
The figure shows the percentage of primary studies that are covered by newspapers depending on the study type (lifestyle versus non-lifestyle). Studies of the lifestyle category described associations linking a pathology to a risk factor on which each subject can act. Regarding non-lifestyle articles, the figure also contrasts initial articles with subsequent ones. Differences in the media coverage between initial studies and subsequent ones were statistically significant (see text) except for studies published in prestigious journals (IF ≥ 30). Raw data are given in Supporting Information (S2 Text).
Fig 2
Fig 2. Replication validity of primary articles reported by newspapers.
The figure shows the percentage of primary articles echoed by newspapers whose main finding was consistent with the corresponding meta-analysis. We considered here the same three categories as in Fig 1: primary articles of the lifestyle category and initial or subsequent non-lifestyle studies. Raw data are given in Supporting Information (S2 Text).
Fig 3
Fig 3. Replication validity of primary studies reported by newspapers in three biomedical domains.
The blue bars show the percentage of primary studies covered by newspapers whose main finding was consistent with the corresponding meta-analysis. The red bars show the percentage of initial findings among primary studies echoed by newspapers and related to four psychiatric disorders (PSY), four neurological diseases (NEURO) and four somatic diseases (SOMA). Raw data are given in Supporting Information (S2 Text).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ioannidis JP. Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med. 2005;2(8):e124 10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Sarewitz D. Beware the creeping cracks of bias. Nature. 2012;485(7397):149 10.1038/485149a - DOI - PubMed
    1. Munafo MR, Stothart G, Flint J. Bias in genetic association studies and impact factor. Mol Psychiatry. 2009;14(2):119–20. 10.1038/mp.2008.77 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Begley CG, Ioannidis JP. Reproducibility in science: improving the standard for basic and preclinical research. Circ Res. 2015;116(1):116–26. 10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.114.303819 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Baker M. 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility. Nature. 2016;533(7604):452–4. 10.1038/533452a - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources