Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Feb 23:14:13.
doi: 10.1186/s12983-017-0197-x. eCollection 2017.

Vocal exchanges during pair formation and maintenance in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata)

Affiliations

Vocal exchanges during pair formation and maintenance in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata)

Pietro Bruno D'Amelio et al. Front Zool. .

Abstract

Background: Pair compatibility affects the success of a pair; however, its causes and mechanisms are not fully understood. Vocal exchange may be very important for pair formation, coordinating pair activities, maintaining the pair bond and mate guarding. To investigate the role of vocal exchange in pair formation and pair maintenance, we explored whether new and established pairs of zebra finches differed in their calling relationships. We used individualised backpack microphones to examine the entire daily vocal emission of pairs, with parallel video recording of behaviour.

Results: We found that in non-breeding, isolated pairs, a specific type of call, the "stack call", was the most common. Furthermore, all pairs used the stack call for precisely timed antiphonal exchange. We confirmed a difference between new and established pairs in social behaviour, with the former spending less time in physical contact. Notably, we found that this was mirrored by a difference in calling behaviour: members of new pairs converged over time on a more symmetric calling relationship. Additionally, we observed different response rates to partners among individuals, but a repeatable relationship of answering within pairs, which may reflect different degrees of motivation to answer the partner.

Conclusions: Our findings show that there is plasticity in calling behaviour and that it changes during pair formation, resulting in a coordinated stack call exchange with a similar number of answers between partners once the pair is established. It is possible that some of the calling relationship measurements that we present reflect pair compatibility.

Keywords: Antiphonal calling; Individualized recording; Monogamous songbirds; Pair compatibility; Vocal communication.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Tools: telemetry transmitter and repertoire. Top picture: a) Different components of the telemetry transmitter. I) Elastic cord, the upper loop encircles the head and the bottom loop goes around the tail. II) Microphone transmitter body and antenna. III) Transmitter case. IV) 1.45 V battery. b) The assembled backpack. c) Zebra finch equipped with a transmitter. Bottom panel: d) Sonograms of the scored calls of the male and the female from a representative experimental pair (male only: song and introductory syllables are not shown): “Tet”; “Stack”; “Distance”, “Kackle”, “Whine”, “Hat”. Despite many generations of captivity, the repertoire is very similar to the one described by Zann (1996) in wild birds. We added one soft call type, called “Hat”. The meaning and function of this call are yet to be determined, but it may be a modified version of the “Thuks” call used to indicate danger in wild populations [28]
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Proportion of call types by day. The proportion of each call type is reported for all the birds of the study. Each row represents a pair and the two columns are for females (left) and males (right). Within each column the 4 days of the study are plotted next to each other. The different colours of the bars represent the different call types (Misp. Intr. is the abbreviation of misplaced introductory syllables: those which were not followed by the song). The height of the bars represents the proportion of call types relative to the bird and day (sum for each bird each day equal to 1). The top 8 pairs with the shaded background are pairs that never met before the experiment whereas the bottom 4 are already established pairs. -Triangles indicate new pairs formed by individuals which had never previously bred successfully with another mate
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Social and calling behaviour over time. Proportion of time spent in physical contact (a) and directionality index (b) of new and established pairs over time. a) Amount of physical contact (“clumping”), given as a proportion of the overall time, for each of the four days. The boxplots represent the row value, the red dots the estimated Bayesian values and the red segments the Credible Intervals (CrI) estimated from the LMM. An asterisk indicates a lack of overlap between CrI and the fitted values (equivalent to frequentist significance set at <5%). b) Directionality index over time. Each line is a pair; established pairs are shown in grey whereas new pairs are in black. The variance in new pairs is bigger at the beginning than at the end, indicating that the relationship becomes quantitatively more symmetrical
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Correlation of the numbers of stack calls and answers between males and females. Total number of stack calls (red dots) and number of stack call replies to stacks (blue crosses) for males and females for each day for each pair (values are normalised dividing by the highest number of calls). Bold lines represent the relationships extracted from the estimated posterior of the linear mixed models; thinner lines are the respective 95% credible intervals. Number of answers is tightly correlated between males and females, while call number is only loosely correlated, indicating that different individuals use differing proportions of calls to answer the partner
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Correlation between proportion of male answers and time spent in physical contact. Amount of time in physical contact and proportion of answers (standardised by z-scores). The dots are the raw values, one point for each male each day, the bold line is the fitted regression line drawn from the posterior distribution of the value estimated from the LMM. The thinner lines and the shaded area represent the credible intervals

References

    1. Nisbet ICT, Dann P. Reproductive performance of little penguins Eudyptula minor in relation to year, age, pair-bond duration, breeding date and individual quality. J Avian Biol. 2009;40:296–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2008.04563.x. - DOI
    1. Ihle M, Kempenaers B, Forstmeier W. Fitness benefits of mate choice for compatibility in a socially monogamous species. PLoS Biol. 2015;13 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002248. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Leu ST, Burzacott D, Whiting MJ, Bull CM. Mate familiarity affects pairing behaviour in a long-term monogamous lizard: evidence from detailed bio-logging and a 31-year field study. Ethology. 2015;121:760–8. doi: 10.1111/eth.12390. - DOI
    1. Sánchez-Macouzet O, Rodríguez C, Drummond H. Better stay together: pair bond duration increases individual fitness independent of age-related variation. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2014;281. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Spoon TR, Millam JR, Owings DH. The importance of mate behavioural compatibility in parenting and reproductive success by cockatiels, Nymphicus hollandicus. Anim Behav. 2006;71:315–26. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.034. - DOI

LinkOut - more resources