Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 May;117(5):955-967.
doi: 10.1007/s00421-017-3584-2. Epub 2017 Mar 14.

Old adults preserve motor flexibility during rapid reaching

Affiliations

Old adults preserve motor flexibility during rapid reaching

Christian Greve et al. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2017 May.

Abstract

Purpose: Our ability to flexibly coordinate the available degrees of freedom allows us to perform activities of daily living under various task constraints. Healthy old adults exhibit subclinical peripheral and central nervous system dysfunctions, possibly compromising the flexibility in inter-joint coordination during voluntary movements and the ability to adapt to varying task constraints.

Method: We examined how healthy old (75.4 ± 5.2 years, n = 14) compared with young adults (24.3 ± 2 years, n = 15) make use of the available motor flexibility to adapt to physical and dexterity constraints during a rapid goal-directed reaching task. We manipulated physical and dexterity demands by changing, respectively, external resistance and target size. Motor flexibility was quantified by an uncontrolled manifold (UCM) analysis.

Results: We found that healthy young and old adults employ similar motor flexibility as quantified by the ratio between goal equivalent and non-goal equivalent variability (V Ratio) and were similarly able to adapt to increases in physical and dexterity demands during goal-directed rapid reaching (V Ratio: p = .092; young: 0.548 ± 0.113; old: 0.264 ± 0.117). Age affected end-effector kinematics. Motor flexibility and end-effector kinematics did not correlate.

Conclusion: The data challenge the prevailing view that old age affects movement capabilities in general and provide specific evidence that healthy old adults preserve motor flexibility during a reaching task. Future studies applying UCM analysis should examine if experimental set-ups limit movement exploration, leaving possible age differences undetected.

Keywords: Ageing; Motor control; Motor flexibility; Reaching; Task demand; Uncontrolled manifold.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the ethics committee of the Center for Human Movement Sciences, University Medical Center Groningen and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Experimental set-up. Participants sat in an adjustable chair in front of a table so that the olecranon process with the elbow flexed at 90° was at tabletop height. The start posture was approximately 20° shoulder abduction, 90° elbow flexion and 90° pronation. To have a consistent start position, participants placed their right olecranon on an elbow support and the pointer tip in a pre-defined start position. The elbow support was positioned at the right side of the participants’ body at the same height with the table. The start position of the pointer tip was marked on the table with a dot of the size of the diameter of the pointer tip. During the start position the back of the pointer tool was placed against a wooden bar to release the load (Greve et al. 2015)
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Time normalized joint position data of the ID 4 and 0-kg condition for young and old participants. The blue line gives the mean, the dashed green line gives the mean of the within participant standard deviation and the red dashed line gives the standard error of the mean of the time normalized joint position data in degrees of the shoulder, elbow and wrist joint. The left panel gives the time normalized joint position data of the young and the right panel of the old participants. (Color figure online)
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
GEV and NGEV (log transformed) averaged across age and dexterity constraints for both physical demand conditions. Vertical bars denote standard error of the mean
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Effective target width for young and old participants and both dexterity demand conditions averaged across physical constraints. Vertical bars denote standard error of the mean
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Effective target width for young and old participants and both physical demand conditions averaged across dexterity demand. Vertical bars denote standard error of the mean

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Bakeman R. Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behav. Res. Methods. 2005;37:379–384. doi: 10.3758/BF03192707. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Baloh RW, Ying SH, Jacobson KM. A longitudinal study of gait and balance dysfunction in normal older people. Arch Neurol. 2003;60:835–839. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.6.835. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bassey EJ, Fiatarone MA, O’Neill EF, Kelly M, Evans WJ, Lipsitz LA. Leg extensor power and functional performance in very old men and women. Clin. Sci. (Lond) 1992;82:321–327. doi: 10.1042/cs0820321. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Beijersbergen CMI, Granacher U, Vandervoort AA, DeVita P, Hortobágyi T. The biomechanical mechanism of how strength and power training improves walking speed in old adults remains unknown. Ageing Res Rev. 2013;12:618–627. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2013.03.001. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bock O. Components of sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly subjects. Exp Brain Res. 2005;160:259–263. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2133-5. - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources