Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2014 Jan 1;22(9-10):1216-1232.
doi: 10.1080/13506285.2014.990545.

Effects of Peripheral Eccentricity and Head Orientation on Gaze Discrimination

Affiliations

Effects of Peripheral Eccentricity and Head Orientation on Gaze Discrimination

Adam Palanica et al. Vis cogn. .

Abstract

Visual search tasks support a special role for direct gaze in human cognition, while classic gaze judgment tasks suggest the congruency between head orientation and gaze direction plays a central role in gaze perception. Moreover, whether gaze direction can be accurately discriminated in the periphery using covert attention is unknown. In the present study, individual faces in frontal and in deviated head orientations with a direct or an averted gaze were flashed for 150 ms across the visual field; participants focused on a centred fixation while judging the gaze direction. Gaze discrimination speed and accuracy varied with head orientation and eccentricity. The limit of accurate gaze discrimination was less than ±6° eccentricity. Response times suggested a processing facilitation for direct gaze in fovea, irrespective of head orientation, however, by ±3° eccentricity, head orientation started biasing gaze judgments, and this bias increased with eccentricity. Results also suggested a special processing of frontal heads with direct gaze in central vision, rather than a general congruency effect between eye and head cues. Thus, while both head and eye cues contribute to gaze discrimination, their role differs with eccentricity.

Keywords: covert attention; face perception; gaze discrimination; peripheral vision; spatial attention.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Schematic representation of stimulus presentation. The dotted rectangles are shown to represent all of the 9 possible locations of stimuli presentation, but were invisible during trials. Negative (−) eccentricities represent target positions to the left of fixation, while positive (+) eccentricities represent those to the right of fixation. The fixation cross was shown during the entire duration of each trial to keep participants’ fixation focused. Please also note that for averted gaze faces, both left- and right-looking faces were used, and for deviated head views, both left- and right-facing head orientations were used.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Results, as a function of gaze direction and eccentricity, for (A) RT responses, (B) Error rates, and (C) Response presses (all shown with standard error bars). Please note that for (B) and (C), gaze direction data were computed within each head orientation (i.e., DG vs. AG for frontal heads, and DG vs. AG for deviated heads). DG vs. AG paired comparisons: stars above lines represent deviated head comparisons; stars below lines represent frontal head comparisons; *p < .001.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Allison T, Puce A, McCarthy G. Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2000;4:267–278. - PubMed
    1. Anstis SM, Mayhew JW, Morley T. The perception of where a face or television “portrait” is looking. American Journal of Psychology. 1969;82:474–489. - PubMed
    1. Baron-Cohen S. How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mind reading. Cahiers de Psychology Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition. 1994;13:513–552.
    1. Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38:813–822. - PubMed
    1. Batki A, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Connellan J, Ahluwalia J. Is there an innate module? Evidence from human neonates. Infant Behavior and Development. 2000;23:223–229.

LinkOut - more resources