Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Apr 25;114(17):4348-4353.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1611898114. Epub 2017 Apr 10.

Eight-minute self-regulation intervention raises educational attainment at scale in individualist but not collectivist cultures

Affiliations

Eight-minute self-regulation intervention raises educational attainment at scale in individualist but not collectivist cultures

René F Kizilcec et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. .

Abstract

Academic credentials open up a wealth of opportunities. However, many people drop out of educational programs, such as community college and online courses. Prior research found that a brief self-regulation strategy can improve self-discipline and academic outcomes. Could this strategy support learners at large scale? Mental contrasting with implementation intentions (MCII) involves writing about positive outcomes associated with a goal, the obstacles to achieving it, and concrete if-then plans to overcome them. The strategy was developed in Western countries (United States, Germany) and appeals to individualist tendencies, which may reduce its efficacy in collectivist cultures such as India or China. We tested this hypothesis in two randomized controlled experiments in online courses (n = 17,963). Learners in individualist cultures were 32% (first experiment) and 15% (second experiment) more likely to complete the course following the MCII intervention than a control activity. In contrast, learners in collectivist cultures were unaffected by MCII. Natural language processing of written responses revealed that MCII was effective when a learner's primary obstacle was predictable and surmountable, such as everyday work or family obligations but not a practical constraint (e.g., Internet access) or a lack of time. By revealing heterogeneity in MCII's effectiveness, this research advances theory on self-regulation and illuminates how even highly efficacious interventions may be culturally bounded in their effects.

Keywords: culture; education; field experiment; goal pursuit; motivation.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
MCII raises course completion only among learners in individualist countries. Course completion outcomes of two randomized field experiments evaluating the effect of mental contrasting (MC), implementation intentions (II), and mental contrasting with implementation intentions (MCII) relative to a control activity in three cultural contexts defined by the Hofstede collectivism–individualism index: collectivist countries [0, 33], balanced countries [34, 66], and individualist countries [67, 100]. Error bars represent ±1 SE.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
MCII’s effect is conditional on the type of learners’ stated obstacle. Course completion outcomes in the first experiment examined by obstacle type and cultural context (n = 9,619). Three obstacle types were identified using natural language processing (latent Dirichlet allocation topic modeling) of learners’ stated obstacles. Error bars represent ±1 SE.
Fig. S1.
Fig. S1.
Course persistence in terms of the percentage of lectures watched in two randomized experiments. The figure shows the distribution density of course persistence for each experimental condition (line color) in three cultural contexts (horizontal facets). Cultural context is defined at a national level based on the Hofstede collectivism–individualism index: collectivist countries [0, 33], balanced countries [34, 66], and individualist countries [67, 100]. In the first experiment, the density lines are visually indistinguishable due to high zero-inflation and overdispersion.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Kena G, et al. 2015. The Condition of Education 2015 (US Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Washington, DC), NCES 2015-144, pp 234–237.
    1. Shah D. 2015 MOOCs in 2015: Breaking down the numbers. EdSurge. Available at https://www.edsurge.com/news/2015-12-28-moocs-in-2015-breaking-down-the-.... Accessed June 1, 2016.
    1. Kizilcec RF, Schneider E. Motivation as a lens to understand online learners: Toward data-driven design with the OLEI scale. ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. 2015;22(2):6.
    1. Waldrop MM. Online learning: Campus 2.0. Nature. 2013;495(7440):160–163. - PubMed
    1. Kizilcec RF, Piech C, Schneider E. Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Learning Analytics and Knowledge, LAK 2013, Leuven, Belgium. ACM; New York: 2013. Deconstructing disengagement: Analyzing learner subpopulations in massive open online courses; pp. 170–179.

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources