Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Randomized Controlled Trial
. 2018;47(sup1):S341-S353.
doi: 10.1080/15374416.2017.1310045. Epub 2017 Apr 17.

Behavioral Parent Training in Infancy: What About the Parent-Infant Relationship?

Affiliations
Randomized Controlled Trial

Behavioral Parent Training in Infancy: What About the Parent-Infant Relationship?

Angela M Blizzard et al. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2018.

Abstract

Behavioral parent training (BPT) and attachment interventions have demonstrated efficacy in improving outcomes for young children. Despite theoretical overlap in these approaches, the literature has evolved separately, particularly with respect to outcome measurement in BPT. We examined the impact of the Infant Behavior Program (IBP), a brief home-based adaptation of Parent-Child Interaction Therapy, on changes in attachment-based caregiving behaviors (sensitivity, warmth, and intrusiveness) at postintervention and 3- and 6-month follow-ups during a videotaped infant-led play. Sixty mother-infant dyads were randomly assigned to receive the IBP (n = 28) or standard care (n = 30). Infants were an average age of 13.52 months and predominately from ethnic or racial minority backgrounds (98%). We used bivariate correlations to examine the association between attachment-based caregiving behaviors and behaviorally based parenting do and don't skills and structural equation modeling to examine the direct effect of the IBP on attachment-based caregiving behaviors and the indirect effect of behaviorally based parenting skills on the relation between intervention group and attachment-based caregiving behaviors. Behaviorally based parenting do and don't skills were moderately correlated with attachment-based caregiving behaviors. Results demonstrated a direct effect of the IBP on warmth and sensitivity at postintervention and 3- and 6-month follow-ups. The direct effect of the IBP on warmth and sensitivity at the 3- and 6-month follow-ups was mediated by increases in parenting do skills at postintervention. Findings suggest that behaviorally based parenting skills targeted in BPT programs have a broader impact on important attachment-based caregiving behaviors during the critical developmental transition from infancy to toddlerhood.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Indirect effects of parenting skills on relation between Infant Behavior Program and responsive caregiving at 3-month follow-up. Note: Values outside parentheses represent standardized regression weights and values inside parentheses represent unstandardized regression weights; dotted line represents indirect effect. Model fit indices: CMIN χ2 = 2.77, p = .964; root mean square error of approximation < .001, PCLOSE p value = .93; comparative fit index = 1.00, Tucker-Lewis index = 1.00. Regression weights for nonsignificant covariates: maternal education, β = 0.03 (0.24); infant age in months, β = −0.10 (−0.08); infant ethnicity, β = 0.004 (0.02); baseline maternal warmth/positive affect, β = 0.14 (0.14); baseline maternal sensitivity/responsivity, β = .009 (.010); and baseline parenting do skills, β = 0.09 (0.02). *p < .05. ***p < .001.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Indirect effects of parenting skills on relation between Infant Behavior Program and responsive caregiving at 6-month follow-up. Note: Values outside the parentheses represent standardized regression weights and values inside the parentheses represent unstandardized regression weights; dotted line represents indirect effect. Model fit indices: CMIN χ2 = 2.58, p = .784; root mean square error of approximation < .001, PCLOSE: p = .845; comparative fit index = 1.00, Tucker-Lewis index = 1.00. Regression weights for covariates: maternal education, β = −0.10 (−0.08); infant age in months, β = −0.13 (−0.10); infant ethnicity, β = 0.08 (0.41); baseline maternal warmth/positive affect, β = 0.10 (0.09); baseline maternal sensitivity/responsivity, β = .014 (.015); and baseline parenting do skills β = 0.15 (0.036). ***p < .001.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall SN. Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1978.
    1. Allen B. A RADical idea: A call to eliminate “attachment disorder” and “attachment therapy” from the clinical lexicon. Evidence-Based Practice in Child and Adolescent Mental Health. 2016:1–12. doi: 10.1080/23794925.2016.1172945. - DOI
    1. Bagner DM. Father’s role in parent training for children with developmental delay. Journal of Family Psychology. 2013;27:650–657. doi: 10.1037/a0033465. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bagner DM, Coxe S, Hungerford GM, Garcia D, Barroso NE, Hernandez J, Rosa-Olivares J. Behavioral parent training in infancy: A window of opportunity for high-risk families. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2016;44:901–912. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0089-5. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bagner DM, Eyberg SM. Father involvement in parent training: When does it matter? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2003;32:599–605. doi:10.1207/s15374424 jccp3204_13. - PubMed

Publication types