Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Comparative Study
. 2017 May 17;26(2):248-265.
doi: 10.1044/2016_AJSLP-15-0169.

Mapping the Early Language Environment Using All-Day Recordings and Automated Analysis

Affiliations
Comparative Study

Mapping the Early Language Environment Using All-Day Recordings and Automated Analysis

Jill Gilkerson et al. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. .

Abstract

Purpose: This research provided a first-generation standardization of automated language environment estimates, validated these estimates against standard language assessments, and extended on previous research reporting language behavior differences across socioeconomic groups.

Method: Typically developing children between 2 to 48 months of age completed monthly, daylong recordings in their natural language environments over a span of approximately 6-38 months. The resulting data set contained 3,213 12-hr recordings automatically analyzed by using the Language Environment Analysis (LENA) System to generate estimates of (a) the number of adult words in the child's environment, (b) the amount of caregiver-child interaction, and (c) the frequency of child vocal output.

Results: Child vocalization frequency and turn-taking increased with age, whereas adult word counts were age independent after early infancy. Child vocalization and conversational turn estimates predicted 7%-16% of the variance observed in child language assessment scores. Lower socioeconomic status (SES) children produced fewer vocalizations, engaged in fewer adult-child interactions, and were exposed to fewer daily adult words compared with their higher socioeconomic status peers, but within-group variability was high.

Conclusions: The results offer new insight into the landscape of the early language environment, with clinical implications for identification of children at-risk for impoverished language environments.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Daily Language Environment Analysis (LENA) measures by child age. Values shown are daily (12-hr) mean counts with 95% confidence interval (CI), computed cross-sectionally within child month of age.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Daily Language Environment Analysis (LENA) measures by mother's attained education (high school [HS]; General Educational Development [GED]). Values shown are daily (12-hr) mean counts with 95% confidence intervals averaged within participant families and socioeconomic strata.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Median group comparison of daily AWC and CT for least versus most educated parents. Values shown are age-standardized (to M = 100, SD = 15), daily (12-hr) adult word and conversational turn counts averaged within families to illustrate the full range of values within socioeconomic groups. Parents in the upper half of the least educated group demonstrate significantly more talk and engagement with their children than parents in the lower half of the most highly educated group.

References

    1. Accardo P. J., & Capute A. J. (2005). The Capute Scales: Cognitive Adaptive Test/Clinical Linguistic & Auditory Milestone Scale (CAT/CLAMS). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
    1. Akhtar N., Jipson J., & Callanan M. A. (2001). Learning words through overhearing. Child Development, 72, 416–430. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00287 - PubMed
    1. Aragon M., & Yoshinaga-Itano C. (2012). Using Language ENvironment Analysis to improve outcomes for children who are deaf or hard of hearing. Seminars in Speech and Language, 33, 340–353. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0032-1326918 - PubMed
    1. Bornstein M. H., Tamis-LeMonda C. S., Hahn C. S., & Haynes O. M. (2008). Maternal responsiveness to young children at three ages: Longitudinal analysis of a multidimensional, modular, and specific parenting construct. Developmental Psychology, 44, 867–874. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.867 - PubMed
    1. Brown R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

LinkOut - more resources