Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Apr 13:8:588.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00588. eCollection 2017.

The Role of Cognition, Personality, and Trust in Fraud Victimization in Older Adults

Affiliations

The Role of Cognition, Personality, and Trust in Fraud Victimization in Older Adults

Rebecca A Judges et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Older adults are more at risk to become a victim of consumer fraud than any other type of crime (Carcach et al., 2001) but the research on the psychological profiles of senior fraud victims is lacking. To bridge this significant gap, we surveyed 151 (120 female, 111 Caucasian) community-dwelling older adults in Southern Ontario between 60 and 90 years of age about their experiences with fraud. Participants had not been diagnosed with cognitive impairment or a neurological disorder by their doctor and looked after their own finances. We assessed their self-reported cognitive abilities using the MASQ, personality on the 60-item HEXACO Personality Inventory, and trust tendencies using a scale from the World Values Survey. There were no demographic differences between victims and non-victims. We found that victims exhibit lower levels of cognitive ability, lower honesty-humility, and lower conscientiousness than non-victims. Victims and non-victims did not differ in reported levels of interpersonal trust. Subsequent regression analyses showed that cognition is an important component in victimization over and above other social factors. The present findings suggest that fraud prevention programs should focus on improving adults' overall cognitive functioning. Further investigation is needed to understand how age-related cognitive changes affect vulnerability to fraud and which cognitive processes are most important for preventing fraud victimization.

Keywords: aging; cognitive ability; fraud; older adults; scams; victimization.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Mean self-reported scores on the MASQ (cognitive ability questionnaire), comparing victims and non-victims. The overall cognitive ability score ranged from a possible 24 to 120 points. The three subscales ranged from a possible 8 to 40 points. Standard errors are represented by the error bars attached to each column. p < 0.05.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Mean self-reported scores on the HEXACO-PI (personality questionnaire), comparing victims and non-victims. All scores ranged from a possible 10 to 50 points. Standard errors are represented by the error bars attached to each column. p < 0.05.

References

    1. Agarwal S., Mazumder B. (2013). Cognitive abilities and household financial decision making. Am. Econ. J. 5 193–207. 10.1257/app.5.1.193 - DOI
    1. Anderson K. B. (2003). Off the Hook: Reducing Participation in Telemarketing Fraud. Washington, DC: AARP Foundation.
    1. Anderson K. B. (2004). Consumer Fraud in the United States: An FTC Survey. Washington, DC: Federal Trade Commission.
    1. Anderson K. B. (2013). Consumer Fraud in the United States, 2011: The Third FTC Survey. Washington, DC: Federal Trade Commission.
    1. Ashton M. C., Lee K. (2008). The prediction of honesty–humility-related criteria by the HEXACO and five-factor models of personality. J. Res. Pers. 42 1216–1228. 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.03.006 - DOI

LinkOut - more resources