Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Apr 24:8:629.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00629. eCollection 2017.

Neuromyths in Music Education: Prevalence and Predictors of Misconceptions among Teachers and Students

Affiliations

Neuromyths in Music Education: Prevalence and Predictors of Misconceptions among Teachers and Students

Nina Düvel et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

In the last decade, educational neuroscience has become increasingly important in the context of instruction, and its applications have been transformed into new teaching methods. Although teachers are interested in educational neuroscience, communication between scientists and teachers is not always straightforward. Thus, misunderstandings of neuroscientific research results can evolve into so-called neuromyths. The aim of the present study was to investigate the prevalence of such music-related neuromyths among music teachers and music students. Based on an extensive literature research, 26 theses were compiled and subsequently evaluated by four experts. Fourteen theses were selected, of which seven were designated as scientifically substantiated and seven as scientifically unsubstantiated (hereafter labeled as "neuromyths"). One group of adult music teachers (n = 91) and one group of music education students (n = 125) evaluated the theses (forced-choice discrimination task) in two separate online surveys. Additionally, in both surveys person-characteristic variables were gathered to determine possible predictors for the discrimination performance. As a result, identification rates of the seven scientifically substantiated theses were similar for teachers (76%) and students (78%). Teachers and students correctly rejected 60 and 59%, respectively, of the seven neuromyths as scientifically unsubstantiated statements. Sensitivity analysis by signal detection theory revealed a discrimination performance of d' = 1.25 (SD = 1.12) for the group of teachers and d' = 1.48 (SD = 1.22) for the students. Both groups showed a general tendency to evaluate the theses as scientifically substantiated (teachers: c = -0.35, students: c = -0.41). Specifically, buzz words such as "brain hemisphere" or "cognitive enhancement" were often classified as correct. For the group of teachers, the best predictor of discrimination performance was having read a large number of media about educational neuroscience and related topics (R2 = 0.06). For the group of students, the best predictors for discrimination performance were a high number of read media and the hitherto completed number of semesters (R2 = 0.14). Our findings make clear that both teachers and students are far from being experts on topics related to educational neuroscience in music and would therefore benefit from current education-related research in psychology and neuroscience.

Keywords: education; educational neuroscience; music students; music teachers; neuromyths; neuroscience of music.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Flowchart of the selection process of theses.
Figure 2
Figure 2
(A,B) Histogram of sensitivity d' (left) and response bias c (right) for the sample of music teachers (y-axis indicates frequencies).
Figure 3
Figure 3
(A,B) Histogram of sensitivity d' (left) and response bias c (right) for the sample of music students (y-axis indicates frequencies).
Figure 4
Figure 4
Regression tree for sensitivity d' as outcome variable. The only relevant predictor for high vs. low discrimination performance is the number of read media (≤3, >3).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Alexander P. A., Murphy P. K., Greene J. A. (2012). Projecting educational psychology's future from its past and present: A trend analysis, in APA Educational Psychology Handbook: Theories, Constructs, and Critical Issues, Vol. 1, eds Harris K. R., Graham S., Urdan T. C. (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; ), 3–32.
    1. Altenmüller E. O. (2001). How many music centers are in the brain? Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 930, 273–280. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05738.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bangert M., Peschel T., Schlaug G., Rotte M., Drescher D., Hinrichs H., et al. . (2006). Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: evidence from fMRI conjunction. Neuroimage 30, 917–926. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.044 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Chabris C. F. (1999). Prelude or requiem for the “Mozart effect”? Nature 400, 826–827. 10.1038/23608 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Dekker S., Lee N. C., Howard-Jones P., Jolles J. (2012). Neuromyths in education: prevalence and predictors of misconceptions among teachers. Front. Psychol. 3:429. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00429 - DOI - PMC - PubMed