Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Jun 6;114(23):5970-5975.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620542114. Epub 2017 May 22.

Facial appearance affects science communication

Affiliations

Facial appearance affects science communication

Ana I Gheorghiu et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. .

Abstract

First impressions based on facial appearance predict many important social outcomes. We investigated whether such impressions also influence the communication of scientific findings to lay audiences, a process that shapes public beliefs, opinion, and policy. First, we investigated the traits that engender interest in a scientist's work, and those that create the impression of a "good scientist" who does high-quality research. Apparent competence and morality were positively related to both interest and quality judgments, whereas attractiveness boosted interest but decreased perceived quality. Next, we had members of the public choose real science news stories to read or watch and found that people were more likely to choose items that were paired with "interesting-looking" scientists, especially when selecting video-based communications. Finally, we had people read real science news items and found that the research was judged to be of higher quality when paired with researchers who look like "good scientists." Our findings offer insights into the social psychology of science, and indicate a source of bias in the dissemination of scientific findings to broader society.

Keywords: impression formation; science communication; social cognition.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Regression coefficients for studies 1 and 2, and pooled across studies. All predictors were standardized. Error bars show 95% confidence intervals; coefficients with CIs that exclude zero are highlighted in black. P_Age, participant age; P_Female, participant gender; and P_Sci, participant science engagement.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
(Top) (Left) The choice data from study 3 and (Right) the interest ratings from study 4. (Bottom) The corresponding regression coefficients. All predictors were standardized (prior to computing interaction terms). Error bars show 95% confidence intervals; coefficients with CIs that exclude zero are highlighted in black. Fem, female scientist; Int, intercept; Phys, physics news item; Vid, video format.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
(Top) The mean quality ratings from (Left) study 5 and (Right) study 6. (Bottom) The corresponding regression coefficients. All predictors were standardized (prior to computing interaction terms). Error bars show 95% Wald confidence intervals; coefficients with CIs that exclude zero are highlighted in black. HiFace, researcher looks like a good scientist.

References

    1. Lok C. Science funding: Science for the masses. Nature. 2010;465:416–418. - PubMed
    1. Peters HP, et al. Interactions with the mass media. Science. 2008;321:204–205. - PubMed
    1. Castell S, et al. Public Attitudes to Science 2014. Ipsos MORI Social Res Inst; London: 2014.
    1. Scheufele DA. Science communication as political communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:13585–13592. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Satterfield JM, et al. Toward a transdisciplinary model of evidence-based practice. Milbank Q. 2009;87:368–390. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources