Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 May 1;17(5):12.
doi: 10.1167/17.5.12.

Intercepting a moving target: On-line or model-based control?

Affiliations

Intercepting a moving target: On-line or model-based control?

Huaiyong Zhao et al. J Vis. .

Abstract

When walking to intercept a moving target, people take an interception path that appears to anticipate the target's trajectory. According to the constant bearing strategy, the observer holds the bearing direction of the target constant based on current visual information, consistent with on-line control. Alternatively, the interception path might be based on an internal model of the target's motion, known as model-based control. To investigate these two accounts, participants walked to intercept a moving target in a virtual environment. We degraded the target's visibility by blurring the target to varying degrees in the midst of a trial, in order to influence its perceived speed and position. Reduced levels of visibility progressively impaired interception accuracy and precision; total occlusion impaired performance most and yielded nonadaptive heading adjustments. Thus, performance strongly depended on current visual information and deteriorated qualitatively when it was withdrawn. The results imply that locomotor interception is normally guided by current information rather than an internal model of target motion, consistent with on-line control.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Definition of variables for interception of a moving target: heading direction (ϕ), bearing direction of the moving target (ψm), distance of the moving target (dm), and target-heading angle (β = ϕ – ψm).
Figure 2
Figure 2
The target visibility conditions, as viewed from a distance of 3 m.
Figure 3
Figure 3
A top-down view of the experimental scenario. The green target appears, moves at a constant speed in front of a gray backdrop, and passes behind a virtual occluder (dashed line). The participant's path is indicated by the blue curve, and the red dot indicates their position when the target moves behind the occluder and is blurred; note that steering adjustments continue after the blur point.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Four predictions about constant and variable interception error due to increasingly degraded target visibility: on-line control hypothesis (green), high-fidelity internal model (red), medium- or low-fidelity internal model (black), and continuously updated internal model (blue).
Figure 5
Figure 5
Sample interception paths from one participant, from no-blur (black traces) and occlusion (red traces) conditions, for target speeds of (a) 0.6 m/s and (b) 1.0 m/s. Black circles represent the target's final position in the no-blur condition, and red circles the same in the occlusion condition. Green circles and lines represent the target's starting position and path. Asterisks on participant paths indicate the blur point when the target went behind the occluder.
Figure 6
Figure 6
(a) Constant error and (b) variable error of interception in each experimental condition. Error bars represent the SE.
Figure 7
Figure 7
(a) Mean interception point (final x position) and (b) mean walking speed after the blur point in each experimental condition. Error bars represent the SE.
Figure 8
Figure 8
Turning rate before blur point (a) and after blur point (b). Positive values represent turning in the target movement direction. Error bars represent the SE.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Bastin J, Craig C, Montagne G. Prospective strategies underlie the control of interceptive actions. Human Movement Sciences. (2006);25:718–732. - PubMed
    1. Bastin J, Fajen B. R, Montagne G. Controlling speed and direction during interception: An affordance-based approach. Experimental Brain Research. (2010);201:763–780. - PubMed
    1. Bastin J, Jacobs D. M, Morice A. H. P, Craig C, Montagne G. Testing the role of expansion in the prospective control of locomotion. Experimental Brain Research. (2008);191:301–312. - PubMed
    1. Bennett S. J, Ashford D, Elliott D. Intermittent vision and one-handed catching. The temporal limits of binocular and monocular integration, Motor Control. (2003);7:378–387. - PubMed
    1. Bennett S. J, Elliott D, Weeks D. J, Keil D. The effects of intermittent vision on prehension under binocular and monocular viewing. Motor Control. (2003);7:46–56. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources