Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2018 May:102:107-120.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.06.018. Epub 2017 Jul 4.

Habitual versus goal-driven attention

Affiliations
Review

Habitual versus goal-driven attention

Yuhong V Jiang. Cortex. 2018 May.

Abstract

Recent research has expanded the list of factors that control spatial attention. Beside current goals and perceptual salience, statistical learning, reward, motivation and emotion also affect attention. But do these various factors influence spatial attention in the same manner, as suggested by the integrated framework of attention, or do they target different aspects of spatial attention? Here I present evidence that the control of attention may be implemented in two ways. Whereas current goals typically modulate where in space attention is prioritized, search habits affect how one moves attention in space. Using the location probability learning paradigm, I show that a search habit forms when people frequently find a visual search target in one region of space. Attentional cuing by probability learning differs from that by current goals. Probability cuing is implicit and persists long after the probability cue is no longer valid. Whereas explicit goal-driven attention codes space in an environment-centered reference frame, probability cuing is viewer-centered and is insensitive to secondary working memory load and aging. I propose a multi-level framework that separates the source of attentional control from its implementation. Similar to the integrated framework, the multi-level framework considers current goals, perceptual salience, and selection history as major sources of attentional control. However, these factors are implemented in two ways, controlling where spatial attention is allocated and how one shifts attention in space.

Keywords: Implicit learning; Search habit; Spatial attention; Spatial reference frame.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Location probability learning. Left: The training-testing two-phase location probability design. In the training phase the target more often appears in one visual quadrant (the “rich”, high-probability quadrant). In the testing phase, the target’s location is random. Right: Reaction time (RT) becomes faster when the target appears in the high-probability quadrant. This effect persists during the testing phase. Each phase has about 500 trials in the experiment depicted here (adapted from Jiang, Swallow, Rosenbaum, & Herzig, 2013).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Experimental setup for testing the persistence of probability cuing after a viewpoint change. Participants perform the T-among-L task on a display laid flat on a desk. Testing phase RT data (in milliseconds) show evidence for viewer-centered, but not environment-centered, coding of the frequently attended locations (adapted from Jiang & Swallow, 2013b; Twedell, Koutstaal, & Jiang, 2016).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Left. Experimental setup used in the study where people stand at variable viewpoints around a display monitor. Right. An illustration of three trials viewed from above. The green footprint indicates the participants’ standing position on that trial. The 50% region indicates where the target is most often placed from that viewpoint. In this setup, the high-probability quadrant is in the same region of the monitor (away from the “red-wall” landmark; adapted from Jiang, Swallow, & Capistrano, 2013).
Figure 4
Figure 4
The dual-system view of attentional implementation (adapted from Jiang, Swallow, & Capistrano, 2013).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Allport A. Foundations of cognitive science. Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press; 1989. Visual attention; pp. 631–682.
    1. Anderson BA, Kuwabara H, Wong DF, Gean EG, Rahmim A, Braŝić JR, Yantis S. The Role of Dopamine in Value-Based Attentional Orienting. Current Biology: CB. 2016;26(4):550–555. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.062. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Awh E, Jonides J. Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5(3):119–126. - PubMed
    1. Awh Edward, Belopolsky AV, Theeuwes J. Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16(8):437–443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Basso MA, Wurtz RH. Modulation of neuronal activity in superior colliculus by changes in target probability. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1998;18(18):7519–7534. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources