Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Jul 24;7(1):6278.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-06541-0.

Digging up food: excavation stone tool use by wild capuchin monkeys

Affiliations

Digging up food: excavation stone tool use by wild capuchin monkeys

Tiago Falótico et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

Capuchin monkeys at Serra da Capivara National Park (SCNP) usually forage on the ground for roots and fossorial arthropods, digging primarily with their hands but also using stone tools to loosen the soil and aid the digging process. Here we describe the stone tools used for digging by two groups of capuchins on SCNP. Both groups used tools while digging three main food resources: Thiloa glaucocarpa tubers, Ocotea sp roots, and trapdoor spiders. One explanation for the occurrence of tool use in primates is the "necessity hypothesis", which states that the main function of tool use is to obtain fallback food. We tested for this, but only found a positive correlation between plant food availability and the frequency of stone tools' use. Thus, our data do not support the fallback food hypothesis for the use of tools to access burrowed resources.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Male capuchin monkey digging with a DST.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Food items searched by the monkeys with the aid of DSTs. Unidentified resources correspond to episodes in which we were unable to determine the target resource even after site inspection.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Thiloa glaucocarpa underground storage organs (USOs). Scale: 10 cm.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Louro tree root (Ocotea sp), with peeled skin. Scale: 10 cm.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Capuchin monkey digging site. (a) Excavated tunnel and the DST used. (b) Detail of the web tunnel. (c) Trapdoor spider (Actinopus sp).
Figure 6
Figure 6
Efficiency in the acquisition of resources using DSTs. Only episodes with resource identification are accounted. (n juveniles = 628; n adults/subadults = 613). * GLMM, p < 0.05.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Arthropods availability (orange line), plant matter availability (green line), and rain precipitation (bars) during the research period.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Falótico T, Ottoni EB. The manifold use of pounding stone tools by wild capuchin monkeys of Serra da Capivara National Park, Brazil. Behaviour. 2016;153:421–442. doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003357. - DOI
    1. De Moraes BLC, Da Silva Souto A, Schiel N. Adaptability in stone tool use by wild capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) Am. J. Primatol. 2014;76:967–977. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22286. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Fragaszy DM, Izar P, Visalberghi E, Ottoni EB, de Oliveira MG. Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools. Am. J. Primatol. 2004;64:359–366. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20085. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Izawa K. Foods and feeding behavior of wild black-capped capuchin (Cebus apella) Primates. 1979;20:57–76. doi: 10.1007/BF02373828. - DOI
    1. Phillips KA, Grafton BW, Haas ME. Tap-scanning for invertebrates by capuchins (Cebus apella) Folia Primatol. 2003;74:162–164. doi: 10.1159/000070650. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources