Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Comparative Study
. 2017 Nov;20(6):1137-1146.
doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1131-5. Epub 2017 Sep 19.

Are parrots poor at motor self-regulation or is the cylinder task poor at measuring it?

Affiliations
Comparative Study

Are parrots poor at motor self-regulation or is the cylinder task poor at measuring it?

Can Kabadayi et al. Anim Cogn. 2017 Nov.

Abstract

The ability to inhibit unproductive motor responses triggered by salient stimuli is a fundamental inhibitory skill. Such motor self-regulation is thought to underlie more complex cognitive mechanisms, like self-control. Recently, a large-scale study, comparing 36 species, found that absolute brain size best predicted competence in motor inhibition, with great apes as the best performers. This was challenged when three Corvus species (corvids) were found to parallel great apes despite having much smaller absolute brain sizes. However, new analyses suggest that it is the number of pallial neurons, and not absolute brain size per se, that correlates with levels of motor inhibition. Both studies used the cylinder task, a detour-reaching test where food is presented behind a transparent barrier. We tested four species from the order Psittaciformes (parrots) on this task. Like corvids, many parrots have relatively large brains, high numbers of pallial neurons, and solve challenging cognitive tasks. Nonetheless, parrots performed markedly worse than the Corvus species in the cylinder task and exhibited strong learning effects in performance and response times. Our results suggest either that parrots are poor at controlling their motor impulses, and hence that pallial neuronal numbers do not always correlate with such skills, or that the widely used cylinder task may not be a good measure of motor inhibition.

Keywords: Brain size; Cylinder task; Detour-reaching task; Inhibition; Motor self-regulation; Psittacidae; Self-control.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical standards

All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. In accordance with the German Animal Welfare Act of 25th May 1998, Section V, Article 7 and the Spanish Animal Welfare Act 32/2007 of 7th November 2007, Preliminary Title, Article 3, the study was classified as non-animal experiment and did not require any approval from a relevant body.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Cylinders used in the study (left: transparent cylinder, right: opaque cylinder)
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Individual learning curves of the 38 birds included in the study, presented separately for each species. Horizontal axes represent the trial numbers, and vertical axes represent the cumulative numbers of correct responses on the cylinder task. The black lines show the individual birds’ scores. The shaded gray areas indicate the upper and lower limits of 95% confidence intervals for the species averages
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Each species’ estimated change in the cylinder task score (proportion correct responses) over the 10 trials, predicted from the generalized linear mixed-effect regression analysis, with correct or incorrect as the binary outcome variable
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Each species’ estimated change in the proportion of ‘away’ failures (i.e., cases where the contact made with the cylinder was directed away from the food, and thus not food-related) over the 10 trials predicted from the generalized mixed model regression analysis, with error type (toward or away) as a binary outcome. The gap in the regression line for the great green macaw is due to the fact that there were no errors at the seventh and eighth trial in this species

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Auersperg AMI, Laumer IB, Bugnyar T. Goffin cockatoos wait for qualitative and quantitative gains but prefer ‘better’ to ‘more’. Biol Lett. 2013;8:20121092. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2012.1092. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Auersperg AMI, von Bayern AMP, Weber S, Szabadvari A, Bugnyar T, Kacelnik A. Social transmission of tool use and tool manufacture in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffini) Proc R Soc B. 2014;281:20140972. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0972. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Beran MJ. The comparative science of ‘self-control’: what are we talking about? Front Psychol. 2015;6:1–4. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Berkman ET, Kahn LE, Merchant JS. Training-induced changes in inhibitory control network activity. J Neurosci. 2014;34:149–157. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3564-13.2014. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Boogert NJ, Anderson RC, Peters S, Searcy WA, Nowicki S. Song repertoire size in male song sparrows correlates with detour reaching, but not with other cognitive measures. Anim Behav. 2011;81:1209–1216. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.03.004. - DOI

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources