Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2018 May;47(5):1052-1072.
doi: 10.1007/s10964-017-0752-y. Epub 2017 Oct 19.

Age Patterns in Risk Taking Across the World

Affiliations

Age Patterns in Risk Taking Across the World

Natasha Duell et al. J Youth Adolesc. 2018 May.

Erratum in

  • Correction to: Age Patterns in Risk Taking Across the World.
    Duell N, Steinberg L, Icenogle G, Chein J, Chaudhary N, Di Giunta L, Dodge KA, Fanti KA, Lansford JE, Oburu P, Pastorelli C, Skinner AT, Sorbring E, Tapanya S, Tirado LMU, Alampay LP, Al-Hassan SM, Takash HMS, Bacchini D, Chang L. Duell N, et al. J Youth Adolesc. 2019 Apr;48(4):835-836. doi: 10.1007/s10964-019-00999-z. J Youth Adolesc. 2019. PMID: 30820728

Abstract

Epidemiological data indicate that risk behaviors are among the leading causes of adolescent morbidity and mortality worldwide. Consistent with this, laboratory-based studies of age differences in risk behavior allude to a peak in adolescence, suggesting that adolescents demonstrate a heightened propensity, or inherent inclination, to take risks. Unlike epidemiological reports, studies of risk taking propensity have been limited to Western samples, leaving questions about the extent to which heightened risk taking propensity is an inherent or culturally constructed aspect of adolescence. In the present study, age patterns in risk-taking propensity (using two laboratory tasks: the Stoplight and the BART) and real-world risk taking (using self-reports of health and antisocial risk taking) were examined in a sample of 5227 individuals (50.7% female) ages 10-30 (M = 17.05 years, SD = 5.91) from 11 Western and non-Western countries (China, Colombia, Cyprus, India, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the US). Two hypotheses were tested: (1) risk taking follows an inverted-U pattern across age groups, peaking earlier on measures of risk taking propensity than on measures of real-world risk taking, and (2) age patterns in risk taking propensity are more consistent across countries than age patterns in real-world risk taking. Overall, risk taking followed the hypothesized inverted-U pattern across age groups, with health risk taking evincing the latest peak. Age patterns in risk taking propensity were more consistent across countries than age patterns in real-world risk taking. Results suggest that although the association between age and risk taking is sensitive to measurement and culture, around the world, risk taking is generally highest among late adolescents.

Keywords: Adolescents; Cross-national; Development; Risk taking.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Age differences in risk taking propensity and real-world risk taking. Note: Self-reported health risk taking = drinking alcohol, getting in the car with a drunk driver, smoking cigarettes, and having unprotected sex; self-reported antisocial risk taking = vandalizing, stealing, fighting, walking through a dangerous neighborhood, and threatening someone. The model was estimated separately for each measure. Slopes represent Y-standardized estimated regression coefficients (centered at the slope for 10-year-olds) for age and age2 adjusted for country, gender, parental education, and intellectual ability. Slopes were standardized specifically for this figure so that measures could be interpreted on the same scale. The quadratic effect of age is significant for all measures.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Age differences in risk taking propensity on the Stoplight across countries. Note: Slopes represent estimated regression coefficients (centered at the slope for 10-year-olds) for age and age2 adjusted for gender, parental education, and intellectual ability. The model was estimated separately for each country. * Countries for which there was a significant quadratic effect of age.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Age differences in risk taking propensity on the BART across countries. Note: Slopes represent estimated regression coefficients (centered at slope for 10-year-olds) for age and age2 adjusted for gender, parental education, and intellectual ability. The model was estimated separately for each country. * Countries for which there was a significant quadratic effect of age.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Age patterns in self-reported health risk taking across countries. Note: Values are percentage (%) of health risks (drinking alcohol, getting in the car with a drunk driver, smoking cigarettes, and having unprotected sex) endorsed. Slopes represent estimated regression coefficients (centered at the slope for 10-year-olds) for age and age2 adjusted for gender, parental education, and intellectual ability. The model was estimated separately for each country. * Countries for which there was a significant quadratic effect of age.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Age patterns in self-reported antisocial risk taking across countries. Note: Values are percentage (%) of antisocial risks (vandalizing, stealing, fighting, walking through a dangerous neighborhood, and threatening someone) endorsed. Slopes represent estimated regression coefficients (centered at the slope 10-year-olds) for age and age2 adjusted for gender, parental education, and intellectual ability. The model was estimated separately for each country. * Countries for which there was a significant quadratic effect of age. ** The quadratic effect is significant for females, but not males.

References

    1. Anderson Johnson C, Palmer PH, Chou CP, Pang Z, Zhou D, Dong L, Xiang H, Yang P, Xu H, Unger JB. Tobacco use among youth and adults in Mainland China: the China Seven Cities Study. Public Health. 2006;120:1156–1169. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2006.07.023. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Anteghini M, Fonseca H, Ireland M, Blum R. Health risk behaviors and associated risk protective factors among Brazilian adolescents in Santos, Brazil. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2001;28:295–302. doi: 10.1016/S1054-139X(00)00197-X. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Casey BJ, Jones R, Somerville L. Braking and accelerating of the adolescent brain. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2011;21:21–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00712.x. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Choudhury S. Culturing the adolescent brain: What can neuroscience learn from anthropology? Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2010;5:159–167. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp030. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Defoe IN, Semon Dubas J, Figner B, van Aken MAG. A meta-analysis on age differences in risky decision making: Adolescents versus children and adults. Psychological Bulletin. 2015;141:48–84. doi: 10.1037/a0038088. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources