Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2018 Feb;80(2):426-438.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1448-9.

Spatiotemporal competition and task-relevance shape the spatial distribution of emotional interference during rapid visual processing: Evidence from gaze-contingent eye-tracking

Affiliations

Spatiotemporal competition and task-relevance shape the spatial distribution of emotional interference during rapid visual processing: Evidence from gaze-contingent eye-tracking

Briana L Kennedy et al. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2018 Feb.

Abstract

People's ability to perceive rapidly presented targets can be disrupted both by voluntary encoding of a preceding target and by spontaneous attention to salient distractors. Distinctions between these sources of interference can be found when people search for a target in multiple rapid streams instead of a single stream: voluntary encoding of a preceding target often elicits subsequent perceptual lapses across the visual field, whereas spontaneous attention to emotionally salient distractors appears to elicit a spatially localized lapse, giving rise to a theoretical account suggesting that emotional distractors and subsequent targets compete spatiotemporally during rapid serial visual processing. We used gaze-contingent eye-tracking to probe the roles of spatiotemporal competition and memory encoding on the spatial distribution of interference caused by emotional distractors, while also ruling out the role of eye-gaze in driving differences in spatial distribution. Spontaneous target perception impairments caused by emotional distractors were localized to the distractor location regardless of where participants fixated. But when emotional distractors were task-relevant, perceptual lapses occurred across both streams while remaining strongest at the distractor location. These results suggest that spatiotemporal competition and memory encoding reflect a dual-route impact of emotional stimuli on target perception during rapid visual processing.

Keywords: Attentional blink; Attentional capture; Visual awareness.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Alternative account predictions
Predicted results according to an account that the spatial specific impairment in emotion-induced blindness results from participants fixating at only one stream at a time. The streams in which targets appear are represented with a T. See text for details.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Schematic of a partial trial in Experiment 1
Participants reported the direction of the one rotated picture that appeared in either of two simultaneously presented RSVP streams (presentation rate 100ms/frame). The distractor item - a colored negative image or colored scene image - appeared either one frame or two frames before the target, which was also colored. All other images in the stream were grayscale. The distractor appeared either in the same stream or opposite stream from the target.
Figure 3
Figure 3. Experiment 1 target accuracy
In Experiment 1, impairment from emotional distractors was localized both when the distractor was fixated and non-fixated. EIB was observed when targets appeared in the same stream as distractors – regardless of whether the participants were fixating on the distractor stream or when fixating at the opposite stream. When the target and distractor appeared in opposite streams, no EIB was observed. Error bars represent standard error, and Ts represent the stream in which the targets appeared.
Figure 4
Figure 4. Experiment 2 target accuracy
In Experiment 2, EIB was observed when targets appeared in the same stream as distractors – both when participants fixated at the distractor stream and when fixating at the opposite stream. When the target and distractor appeared in opposite streams, EIB was also observed. However, the emotion-induced impairment was greater when distractors and targets appeared in the same stream, compared to when they appeared in opposite streams. Error bars represent standard error, and Ts represent the streams in which the targets appeared.
Figure 5
Figure 5. Experiment 2 distractor memory performance
Participants remembered distractors better when they were negative compared to when they were featural distractors. They also remembered distractors better when the distractors were fixated, compared to non-fixated. Error bars represent standard error, and Ts represent the streams in which the targets appeared.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Arnell KM, Killman KV, Fijavz D. Blinded by emotion: target misses follow attention capture by arousing distractors in RSVP. Emotion. 2007;7(3):465–477. http://doi.org/10.1167/4.8.359. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Brainard DH. The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision. 1997;10(4):433–436. http://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Chun MM, Potter MC. A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1995;21(1):109–27. http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.21.1.109. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1995;18(1):193–222. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Deubel H, Schneider WX. Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research. 1996;36:1827–1837. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(95)00294-4. - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources