Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Dec 20;7(1):17898.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18092-5.

Stroboscopic vision and sustained attention during coincidence-anticipation

Affiliations

Stroboscopic vision and sustained attention during coincidence-anticipation

Rafael Ballester et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

We compared coincidence-anticipation performance in normal vision and stroboscopic vision as a function of time-on-task. Participants estimated the arrival time of a real object that moved with constant acceleration (-0.7, 0, +0.7 m/s2) in a pseudo-randomised order across 4 blocks of 30 trials in both vision conditions, received in a counter-balanced order. Participants (n = 20) became more errorful (accuracy and variability) in the normal vision condition as a function of time-on-task, whereas performance was maintained in the stroboscopic vision condition. We interpret these data as showing that participants failed to maintain coincidence-anticipation performance in the normal vision condition due to monotony and attentional underload. In contrast, the stroboscopic vision condition placed a greater demand on visual-spatial memory for motion extrapolation, and thus participants did not experience the typical vigilance decrement in performance. While short-term adaptation effects from practicing in stroboscopic vision are promising, future work needs to consider for how long participants can maintain effortful processing, and whether there are negative carry-over effects from cognitive fatigue when transferring to normal vision.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Temporal course of the stimuli presentation in the PVT.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Group mean constant error as a function of time-on-task (Block 1–4) for the normal vision and stroboscopic vision conditions. Vertical bars represent standard errors of the mean.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Group mean variable error as a function of time-on-task (Block 1–4) for the normal vision and stroboscopic vision conditions. Vertical bars represent standard errors of the mean.

References

    1. Elliott D, Chua R, Pollock BJ. The influence of intermittent vision on manual aiming. Acta Psychol. 1994;85:1–13. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(94)90016-7. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Mitroff SR, Friesen P, Bennett D, Yoo H, Reichow A. Enhancing ice hockey skills through stroboscopic visual training–A pilot study. Athl Train Sports Health Care. 2013;5:261–264. doi: 10.3928/19425864-20131030-02. - DOI
    1. Clark JF, Ellis JK, Bench J, Khoury J, Graman P. High-performance vision training improves batting statistics for University of Cincinnati baseball players. PloS One. 2012;7:e29109. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029109. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Appelbaum, L. G. & Erickson, G. Sports vision training: A review of the state-of-the-art in digital training techniques. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol, 1–30 (2016).
    1. Appelbaum LG, Cain MS, Schroeder JE, Darling EF, Mitroff SR. Stroboscopic visual training improves information encoding in short-term memory. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2012;74:1681–1691. doi: 10.3758/s13414-012-0344-6. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources