Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2017 Dec 11:9:350.
doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00350. eCollection 2017.

Cognitive-Motor Interference during Walking in Older Adults with Probable Mild Cognitive Impairment

Affiliations

Cognitive-Motor Interference during Walking in Older Adults with Probable Mild Cognitive Impairment

Thomas J Klotzbier et al. Front Aging Neurosci. .

Abstract

Although several studies have shown that dual-tasking (DT) mobility is impaired in Alzheimer's disease, studies on the effects of DT conditions in probable Mild Cognitive Impairment (pMCI) have not yielded unequivocal results. The objectives of the study were to (1) examine the effect of a concurrent task on a complex walking task in adults with cognitive impairment; and (2) determine whether the effect varied with different difficulty levels of the concurrent task. Furthermore, the study was designed to evaluate the Trail-Walking Test (TWT) as a potential detection tool for MCI. We examined DT performance in 42 young adults (mean age 23.9 ± 1.98), and 43 older adults (mean age 68.2 ± 6.42). The MoCA was used to stratify the subjects into those with and without pMCI. DT was assessed using the TWT: participants completed 5 trials each of walking along a fixed pathway, stepping on targets with increasing sequential numbers (i.e., 1-2-…-15), and increasing sequential numbers and letters (i.e., 1-A-2-B-3-…-8). Motor and cognitive DT effects (DTE) were calculated for each task. ROC curves were used to distinguish younger and healthy older adults from older adults with pMCI. The TWT showed excellent test-retest reliability across all conditions and groups (ICC : 0.83-0.97). SEM% was also low (<11%) as was the MDC95% (<30%). Within the DT conditions, the pMCI group showed significantly longer durations for all tasks regardless of the cognitive load compared to the younger and the healthy older adults. The motor DTEs were greatest for the complex condition in older adults with pMCI more so than in comparison with younger and healthy older adults. ROC analyses confirmed that only the tasks with higher cognitive load could differentiate older adults with pMCI from controls (area under the curve >0.7, p < 0.05). The TWT is a reliable DT mobility measure in people with pMCI. However, the condition with high cognitive load is more sensitive than the condition with low cognitive load in identifying pMCI. The TWT-3 thus could serve as a screening tool for early detection of individuals with pMCI. Future studies need to determine the neural correlates for cognitive-motor interference in older adults with pMCI.

Keywords: Trail-Walking Test; cognitive reserve; dual task costs; executive attention network; reliability; visuo-spatial working memory.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
(A) Condition 1 (motor condition), (B) Condition 2 (attention, visual scanning, information processing speed) and (C) Condition 3 (cognitive flexibility, inhibition, working memory) of the Trail-Walking Test (length 41 m) (modified after Schott, 2015).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Means and standard deviation for (A) durations and (B) number of errors by group, and condition of the Trail-Walking-Test (**p < 0.01; *p < 0.05; Tp < 0.10; ns, non-significant).
Figure 3
Figure 3
An examination of the dual-task effects of the cognitive and motor conditions on the (A) TWT-2 and the (B) TWT-3 by group reveals that the majority of individuals either experienced mutual interference, where both motor and cognitive performance declined under dual-task conditions, or prioritized cognition, such that motor performance decreased but cognitive performance increased under dual-task conditions.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Patterns of cognitive-motor dual-task interference for the TWT by group (means and standard deviation) (**p < 0.01; *p < 0.05; Tp < 0.10).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Aggarwal N. T., Wilson R. S., Beck T. L., Bienias J. L., Bennett D. A. (2006). Motor dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment and the risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 63, 1763–1769. 10.1001/archneur.63.12.1763 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Albert M. S., DeKosky S. T., Dickson D., Dubois B., Feldman H. H., Fox N. C., et al. . (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the national institute on aging-Alzheimer's association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 270–279. 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Alexander N. B., Ashton-Miller J. A., Giordani B., Guire K., Schultz A. B. (2005). Age differences in timed accurate stepping with increasing cognitive and visual demand: a walking trail making test. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 60, 1558–1562. - PubMed
    1. Allali G., Ayers E. I., Verghese J. (2015). Motoric cognitive risk syndrome subtypes and cognitive profiles. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 71, 378–384. 10.1093/gerona/glv092 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Allali G., Kressig R. W., Assal F., Herrmann F. R., Dubost V., Beauchet O. (2007). Changes in gait while backward counting in demented older adults with frontal lobe dysfunction. Gait Posture 26, 572–576. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.12.011 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources