Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2018 May;42(4):1146-1178.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12593. Epub 2018 Feb 14.

An Exception to Mental Simulation: No Evidence for Embodied Odor Language

Affiliations

An Exception to Mental Simulation: No Evidence for Embodied Odor Language

Laura J Speed et al. Cogn Sci. 2018 May.

Abstract

Do we mentally simulate olfactory information? We investigated mental simulation of odors and sounds in two experiments. Participants retained a word while they smelled an odor or heard a sound, then rated odor/sound intensity and recalled the word. Later odor/sound recognition was also tested, and pleasantness and familiarity judgments were collected. Word recall was slower when the sound and sound-word mismatched (e.g., bee sound with the word typhoon). Sound recognition was higher when sounds were paired with a match or near-match word (e.g., bee sound with bee or buzzer). This indicates sound-words are mentally simulated. However, using the same paradigm no memory effects were observed for odor. Instead it appears odor-words only affect lexical-semantic representations, demonstrated by higher ratings of odor intensity and pleasantness when an odor was paired with a match or near-match word (e.g., peach odor with peach or mango). These results suggest fundamental differences in how odor and sound-words are represented.

Keywords: Audition; Embodiment; Memory; Mental simulation; Olfaction.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Encoding and recognition procedure for (A) Experiment 1 and (B) Experiment 2.
Figure 2
Figure 2
(A) Mean word recall accuracy, (B) LME predicted mean word recall response time, and (C) mean sound recognition. Error bars reflect standard error (A) and model predicted 95% confidence intervals (B and C).
Figure 3
Figure 3
LME predicted mean (A) sound intensity rating, (B) sound pleasantness rating, and (C) sound familiarity rating. Error bars reflect 95% confidence intervals.
Figure 4
Figure 4
(A) LME predicted mean word recall accuracy, (B) word recall response time, and (C) odor recognition accuracy. Error bars reflect 95&percnt ; confidence intervals
Figure 5
Figure 5
LME predicted mean (A) odor intensity ratings, (B) odor pleasantness ratings, and (C) odor familiarity ratings. Error bars denote 95&percnt ; confidence intervals.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Andrade, J. , & Donaldson, L. (2007). Evidence for an olfactory store in working memory? Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 50(2), 76–89. https://doi.org/10.2117/psysoc.2007.76 - DOI
    1. Andrade, J. , May, J. , Deeprose, C. , Baugh, S.‐J. , & Ganis, G. (2014). Assessing vividness of mental imagery: The Plymouth Sensory Imagery Questionnaire. British Journal of Psychology, 105(4), 547–563. - PubMed
    1. Ayabe‐Kanamura, S. , Kikuchi, T. , & Saito, S. (1997). Effect of verbal cues on recognition memory and pleasantness evaluation of unfamiliar odors. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85(1), 275–285. https://doi.org/10.2466/PMS.85.5.275-285 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Barros‐Loscertales, A. , Gonzalez, J. , Pulvermuller, F. , Ventura‐Campos, N. , Bustamante, J. C. , Costumero, V. , Parcet, M. A. , & Avila, C. (2012). Reading salt activates gustatory brain regions: fMRI evidence for semantic grounding in a novel sensory modality. Cerebral Cortex, 22(11), 2554–2563. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr324 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 637–660. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99532147 - DOI - PubMed

Publication types