Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2018 Mar 28;285(1875):20180006.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.0006.

Concede or clash? Solitary sharks competing for food assess rivals to decide

Affiliations

Concede or clash? Solitary sharks competing for food assess rivals to decide

Pierpaolo F Brena et al. Proc Biol Sci. .

Abstract

To adapt to their environment, organisms can either directly interact with their surroundings or use social information (i.e. information provided by neighbouring individuals). Social information relates to the external features of surrounding peers, and little is known about its use by solitary species. Here, we investigated the use of social cues in a solitary marine predator by creating artificial aggregations of free-ranging sicklefin lemon sharks (Negaprion acutidens). Using a novel monitoring protocol, we analysed both dominance interactions and tolerance associations between sharks competing for food in relation with the number, the morphology and the behaviour of rivals. Sharks produced more agonistic displays and spent more time around the bait as competitors were more abundant. Moreover, the morphological attributes of competitors had very limited influence on the structure of shark social interactions. Instead, sharks appeared to establish tolerance relationships with competitors according to their individual behaviour. Furthermore, the more two sharks were observed together at a given study site, the fewer agonistic interactions they exchanged. We discuss these findings as evidence of the use of social cues in a non-gregarious predatory species and suggest directions for future research.

Keywords: dominance hierarchy; heterarchy; in situ experiment; shark; social information.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

We have no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Sicklefin lemon shark tolerance network and dominance hierarchy. (a) Tolerance network. Nodes represent sharks and individuals are labelled as follows: F (females) or M (males) + ID number. Edges indicate the SRI association index. Node size and colour are respectively proportional to shark length and dominance score. (b) Sharks had more connections in the tolerance network (degree) as they showed stronger fidelity to the respective study sites. (c) Sharks produced fewer agonistic behaviours as they showed higher fidelity to the respective study sites. (d) Sharks that showed intermediate submissive rates had more connections in the tolerance network (degree) than sharks that showed high or low submissive rates.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Shark dominance hierarchy. (a) Relationship between the difference in rank and the probability for the higher-ranked individual to win the interaction. Value and significance of the hierarchy transitivity (Ttri) and the ratio of interactions to individuals are also indicated. (b) Dominance interaction matrix. Cell colour captures how often individual sharks (rows) dominate rivals (columns), while controlling for the co-occurrence of individuals.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Behavioural shift within the shark aggregation. Green arrows show the direction of submissive displays pictured in three steps.

References

    1. Danchin É, Giraldeau L-A, Valone TJ, Wagner RH. 2004. Public information: from nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305, 487–491. (10.1126/science.1098254) - DOI - PubMed
    1. Shettleworth SJ. 2010. Cognition, evolution, and behavior, 2nd edn Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
    1. Sumpter DJT. 2006. The principles of collective animal behaviour. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 361, 5–22. (10.1098/rstb.2005.1733) - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Couzin ID. 2009. Collective cognition in animal groups. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 36–43. (10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.002) - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bergman TJ, Beehner JC, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. 2003. Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science 302, 1234–1236. (10.1126/science.1087513) - DOI - PubMed

Publication types