Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2018 Jul:62:34-41.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.04.009. Epub 2018 Apr 30.

On a 'failed' attempt to manipulate visual metacognition with transcranial magnetic stimulation to prefrontal cortex

Affiliations

On a 'failed' attempt to manipulate visual metacognition with transcranial magnetic stimulation to prefrontal cortex

Eugene Ruby et al. Conscious Cogn. 2018 Jul.

Abstract

Rounis, Maniscalco, Rothwell, Passingham, and Lau (2010) reported that stimulation of prefrontal cortex impairs visual metacognition. Bor, Schwartzman, Barrett, and Seth (2017) attempted to replicate this result, but adopted an experimental design that reduced their chanceof obtaining positive findings. Despite that, their results appeared initially consistent with those of Rounis et al., but they subsequently claimed it was necessary to discard ∼30% of their subjects, after which they reported a null result. Using computer simulations, we found that, contrary to their supposed purpose, excluding subjects by Bor et al.'s criteria does not reduce false positive rates. Including both their positive and negative result in a Bayesian framework, we show the correct interpretation is that PFC stimulation likely impaired visual metacognition, exactly contradicting Bor et al.'s claims. That lesion and inactivation studies demonstrate similar positive effects further suggests that Bor et al.'s reported negative finding isn't evidence against the role of prefrontal cortex in metacognition.

Keywords: Consciousness; Metacognition; Prefrontal cortex; Transcranial magnetic stimulation; Visual awareness.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Disclosure of Interest

The authors report no conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Signal detection theoretic framework for the simulated spatial 2AFC task
For a given subject s, each stimulus presentation (either ◼◆or ◆◼) caused an internal response value (x), with X◼◆ ~ N(d’/2, 1) and X◼ ~ N(-d’/2, 1), and the subject then indicated which of the two shape configurations appeared. If x exceeds the subjects Type 1 criterion (cs,1), then the subject responded “◼◆;” otherwise the subject responded “◆◼.” Objective performance capacity (d’) is the normalized distance between the two distributions. The subject then indicated how clearly/confidently they saw the stimulus, based on a comparison between x and the Type 2 criterion (-cs,2 and cs,2). If x < cs,1 or x > cs,2 the subject responded “clear”; otherwise the subject responded “unclear.” If TMS is present and assumed to degrade metacognitive sensitivity, noise (σs,TMS) is added for the Type 2 responses, such that the relevant computations are whether x < cs,1 + εtrial or x > cs,2 + εtrial, with εtrial ~ N(0,σs,TMS) and resampled on each trial (see Supplementary Materials for more details).
Figure 2
Figure 2. Excluding subjects yields little impact on statistical power or false positive rates
As in the actual empirical studies, the effect of TMS was assessed by the statistical significance of the interaction between TMS (DLPFC or control) and time (before TMS and after TMS). (a) Excluding versus not excluding subjects yielded no meaningful change in false positive rate for the between-subjects design (Bor’s Exp. 1). Exclusion led to a small decrease in false positive rate for the double-repeat design (Bor’s Exp. 2), but this design inflated false positive rate in comparison to the between-subjects design (Bor Exp. 1) – although the inflation disappeared when we used permutation tests to address violations of normality assumptions (see main text). (b) Excluding subjects yielded an increase in statistical power compared with not excluding subjects for the between-subjects design (Bor’s Exp. 1), and the magnitude of this increase was even greater when using nonparametric permutation tests to evaluate statistical significance (see main text). Conversely, exclusion resulted in a decrease in power in comparison with no exclusion for the atypical double-repeat within-subjects design (Bor’s Exp. 2). Importantly, the double-repeat design (Bor’s Exp. 2) led to considerably lower rather than the intended higher statistical power when excluding subjects.

Comment in

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Baird B, Smallwood J, Gorgolewski KJ, Margulies DS. Medial and Lateral Networks in Anterior Prefrontal Cortex Support Metacognitive Ability for Memory and Perception. J Neurosci. 2013;33(42):16657–65. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Barrett A, Dienes Z, Seth AK. Measures of metacognition on signal-detection theoretic models. Psychol Methods. 2013;18(4):535–52. - PubMed
    1. Bollen KA, Jackman RW. Regression diagnostics: An expository treatment of outliers and influential cases. Sociol Methods Res. 1985;13(4):510–42.
    1. Bor D, Seth AK. Consciousness and the Prefrontal Parietal Network: Insights from Attention, Working Memory, and Chunking. Front Psychol. 2012;3:63. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bor D, Schwartzman DJ, Barrett AB, Seth AK. Theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation to the prefrontal or parietal cortex does not impair metacognitive visual awareness. PLoS One. 2017;12(2):e0171793. - PMC - PubMed

MeSH terms