Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2018 Jun 19;61(6):1381-1392.
doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-16-0432.

The Receptive-Expressive Gap in English Narratives of Spanish-English Bilingual Children With and Without Language Impairment

Affiliations

The Receptive-Expressive Gap in English Narratives of Spanish-English Bilingual Children With and Without Language Impairment

Todd A Gibson et al. J Speech Lang Hear Res. .

Abstract

Purpose: First, we sought to extend our knowledge of second language (L2) receptive compared to expressive narrative skills in bilingual children with and without primary language impairment (PLI). Second, we sought to explore whether narrative receptive and expressive performance in bilingual children's L2 differed based on the type of contextual support.

Method: In a longitudinal group study, 20 Spanish-English bilingual children with PLI were matched by sex, age, nonverbal IQ score, and language exposure to 20 bilingual peers with typical development and administered the Test of Narrative Language (Gillam & Pearson, 2004) in English (their L2) at kindergarten and first grade.

Results: Standard scores were significantly lower for bilingual children with PLI than those without PLI. An L2 receptive-expressive gap existed for bilingual children with PLI at kindergarten but dissipated by first grade. Using single pictures during narrative generation compared to multiple pictures during narrative generation or no pictures during narrative retell appeared to minimize the presence of a receptive-expressive gap.

Conclusions: In early stages of L2 learning, bilingual children with PLI have an L2 receptive-expressive gap, but their typical development peers do not. Using a single picture during narrative generation might be advantageous for this population because it minimizes a receptive-expressive gap.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Typical development (TD) and primary language impairment (PLI) total narrative performance: Modality × Time. Kinder = kindergarten.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Typical development (TD) and primary language impairment (PLI) narrative performance in the no picture + retell condition: Modality × Time. Kinder = kindergarten.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Typical development (TD) and primary language impairment (PLI) narrative performance in the multiple pictures + generation condition: Modality × Time. Kinder = kindergarten.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Typical development (TD) and primary language impairment (PLI) narrative performance in the single picture + generation condition: Modality × Time. Kinder = kindergarten.

References

    1. Altman C., Armon-Lotem S., Fichman S., & Walters J. (2016). Macrostructure, microstructure, and mental state terms in the narratives of English–Hebrew bilingual preschool children with and without specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37, 165–193.
    1. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Fourth Edition (Text Revision). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
    1. Applebee A. N. (1978). The child's concept of story: Ages two to seventeen. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
    1. Baddeley A. (1999). Essentials of human memory. Essex, UK: Psychology Press.
    1. Barrouillet P., Gavens N., Vergauwe E., Gaillard V., & Camos V. (2009). Working memory span development: A time-based resource-sharing model account. Developmental Psychology, 45, 477–490. - PubMed

Publication types