Cognitive Dissonance, Personalized Feedback, and Online Gambling Behavior: An Exploratory Study Using Objective Tracking Data and Subjective Self-Report
- PMID: 29904326
- PMCID: PMC5986838
- DOI: 10.1007/s11469-017-9808-1
Cognitive Dissonance, Personalized Feedback, and Online Gambling Behavior: An Exploratory Study Using Objective Tracking Data and Subjective Self-Report
Abstract
Providing personalized feedback about the amount of money that gamblers have actually spent may-in some cases-result in cognitive dissonance due to the mismatch between what gamblers actually spent and what they thought they had spent. In the present study, the participant sample (N = 11,829) was drawn from a Norwegian population that had played at least one game for money in the past six months on the Norsk Tipping online gambling website. Players were told that they could retrieve personalized information about the amount of money they had lost over the previous 6-month period. Out of the 11,829 players, 4045 players accessed information about their personal gambling expenditure and were asked whether they thought the amount they lost was (i) more than expected, (ii) about as much as expected, or (iii) less than expected. It was hypothesized that players who claimed that the amount of money lost gambling was more than they had expected were more likely to experience a state of cognitive dissonance and would attempt to reduce their gambling expenditure more than other players who claimed that the amount of money lost was as much as they expected. The overall results contradicted the hypothesis because players without any cognitive dissonance decreased their gambling expenditure more than players experiencing cognitive dissonance. However, a more detailed analysis of the data supported the hypothesis because specific playing patterns of six different types of gambler using a machine-learning tree algorithm explained the paradoxical overall result.
Keywords: Behavioral tracking; Cognitive dissonance; Gambling; Gambling expenditure; Online gambling.
Conflict of interest statement
Compliance with Ethical StandardsThe present study was granted ethical approval by the research team's university ethics committee. All participants gave their informed consent in participating in the survey. The first author’s company (neccton Ltd.) received funding from Norsk Tipping (the gambling operator owned by the Norwegian Government) for this work. The second author was sub-contracted by neccton Ltd. The second author has received funding for a number of research projects in the area of gambling education for young people, social responsibility in gambling, and gambling treatment from GambleAware (formerly the Responsibility in Gambling Trust), a charitable body which funds its research program based on donations from the gambling industry. Both authors undertake consultancy for various gaming companies in the area of social responsibility in gambling.
Similar articles
-
Personalized Behavioral Feedback for Online Gamblers: A Real World Empirical Study.Front Psychol. 2016 Nov 28;7:1875. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01875. eCollection 2016. Front Psychol. 2016. PMID: 27965611 Free PMC article.
-
The use of personalized behavioral feedback for online gamblers: an empirical study.Front Psychol. 2015 Sep 23;6:1406. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01406. eCollection 2015. Front Psychol. 2015. PMID: 26441779 Free PMC article.
-
Self-Reported Losses Versus Actual Losses in Online Gambling: An Empirical Study.J Gambl Stud. 2017 Sep;33(3):795-806. doi: 10.1007/s10899-016-9648-0. J Gambl Stud. 2017. PMID: 27815667 Free PMC article.
-
[Internet gambling: what are the risks?].Encephale. 2012 Feb;38(1):42-9. doi: 10.1016/j.encep.2011.01.014. Epub 2011 Apr 8. Encephale. 2012. PMID: 22381723 Review. French.
-
Psychopathology of Online Poker Players: Review of Literature.J Behav Addict. 2016 Jun;5(2):155-68. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.035. Epub 2016 Jun 27. J Behav Addict. 2016. PMID: 27348559 Free PMC article. Review.
Cited by
-
'We are not the ones to blame'. Gamblers' and providers' appraisal of self-exclusion in Germany.BMC Public Health. 2023 Feb 14;23(1):322. doi: 10.1186/s12889-023-15117-9. BMC Public Health. 2023. PMID: 36788494 Free PMC article.
-
Conceptual Issues Concerning Internet Addiction and Internet Gaming Disorder: Further Critique on Ryding and Kaye (2017).Int J Ment Health Addict. 2018;16(1):233-239. doi: 10.1007/s11469-017-9818-z. Epub 2017 Oct 17. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2018. PMID: 29491772 Free PMC article.
-
Gamblers' Perception of the Playscan Risk Assessment: A Mixed-Methods Study.J Gambl Stud. 2022 Jun;38(2):591-606. doi: 10.1007/s10899-021-10043-0. Epub 2021 Jun 5. J Gambl Stud. 2022. PMID: 34091781 Free PMC article.
-
Digital Traces of Behaviour Within Addiction: Response to Griffiths (2017).Int J Ment Health Addict. 2018;16(1):240-245. doi: 10.1007/s11469-017-9855-7. Epub 2018 Jan 3. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2018. PMID: 29491773 Free PMC article.
-
The Impact of Personalized Feedback Interventions by a Gambling Operator on Subsequent Gambling Expenditure in a Sample of Dutch Online Gamblers.J Gambl Stud. 2023 Jun;39(2):929-946. doi: 10.1007/s10899-022-10162-2. Epub 2022 Nov 9. J Gambl Stud. 2023. PMID: 36352314 Free PMC article.
References
-
- Auer, M., Schneeberger, A. & Griffiths, M. D. (2012). Theoretical loss and gambling intensity: A simulation study. Gaming Law Review and Economics, 16, 269–273.
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources
Other Literature Sources
Miscellaneous