Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2018 Jul 17;8(1):10811.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-29007-3.

Task-specific and variability-driven activation of cognitive control processes during motor performance

Affiliations

Task-specific and variability-driven activation of cognitive control processes during motor performance

Christina Stuhr et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

It has long been postulated that cognitive and motor functions are functionally intertwined. While the idea received convincing support from neuroimaging studies providing evidence that motor and cognitive processes draw on common neural mechanisms and resources, findings from behavioral studies are rather inconsistent. The purpose of the present study was to identify and verify key factors that act on the link between cognitive and motor functions. Specifically we investigated whether it is possible to predict motor skills from cognitive functions. While our results support the idea that motor and cognitive functions are functionally intertwined and different motor skills entail distinct cognitive functions, our data also strongly suggest that the impact of cognitive control processes on motor skill proficiency depends on performance variability, i.e. on how challenging a motor task is. Based on these findings, we presume that motor skills activate specific cognitive control processes on two levels: basic processes that are solely related to the type of the motor task, and variability-driven processes that come into play when performance variability is high. For practitioners, these findings call for specific and challenging motor training interventions to directly tap into the to-be-improved cognitive skills and to involve a maximum of cognitive processes.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests. All procedures performed in this study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Standardized regression coefficients for the relation between specific cognitive control processes, (A) response inhibition, (B) set shifting, (C) response shifting), and balance control as mediated by performance variability (PV) during one-leg balance. Reported are the standardized regression coefficients along with Benjamini-Hochberg adjusted p-values, ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, §p < 0.10, N = 46.

References

    1. Piaget, J. The origins of intelligence in children, 10.1037/11494-000 (W W Norton & Co, 1952).
    1. Bloedel JR, Bracha V. Duality of cerebellar motor and cognitive functions. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 1997;41:613–634. doi: 10.1016/S0074-7742(08)60373-6. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 2000;71:44–56. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00117. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. Rev. 2013;64:135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Leisman G, Moustafa A, Shafir T. Thinking, walking, talking: integratory motor and cognitive brain function. Front. Public Heal. 2016;4:1–19. - PMC - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources