Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2018 Jul 23;3(4):e000752.
doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2018-000752. eCollection 2018.

Early childhood cognitive development is affected by interactions among illness, diet, enteropathogens and the home environment: findings from the MAL-ED birth cohort study

Collaborators

Early childhood cognitive development is affected by interactions among illness, diet, enteropathogens and the home environment: findings from the MAL-ED birth cohort study

MAL-ED Network Investigators. BMJ Glob Health. .

Abstract

Background: Millions of children in low-income and middle-income countries (LMICs) are at risk of not reaching their full cognitive potential. Malnutrition and enteric infections in early life are implicated as risk factors; however, most studies on these risks and their associations with cognitive development have failed to adequately account for confounding factors or the accumulation of putative insults. Here, we examine the interaction between infections and illness on cognitive development in LMIC community settings.

Methods: As part of the Etiology, Risk Factors, and Interactions of Enteric Infections and Malnutrition and the Consequences for Child Health and Development (MAL-ED) longitudinal birth cohort study, children from eight LMICs were followed from birth to 24 months to understand the influence of repeated enteric infections on child growth and development. Here, data from six sites were employed to evaluate associations between infection, illness, the home environment, micronutrient intake and status, maternal reasoning, and cognitive development at 24 months.

Results: Higher rates of enteropathogen detection and days with illness were associated with lower haemoglobin concentrations, which in turn were associated with lower cognitive scores at 24 months. Children with lower environmental health/safety scores and lower intakes of vitamin B6 and folate had more enteropathogen detections and illness. Strength of associations varied by weight-for-age in the first 17 days of life; lower weight infants were more susceptible to the negative effects of enteropathogens and illness.

Conclusions: Enteropathogens were negatively related to child cognitive development. However, other factors were more strongly associated with child cognition. Targeting of interventions to improve cognitive development should include a focus on reducing frequency of illness, improving the safety and healthfulness of the child's environment, and improving dietary intake.

Keywords: anaemia; child health; hygiene; nutrition; parasitology.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Competing interests: None declared.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Path analysis model tested with hypothesised direct relationships between variables (blue arrows indicate negative associations and red arrows indicate positive associations).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Standardised parameter estimates for direct mean effects from tested models. *Parameter fixed to allow for model estimation (blue arrows indicate negative associations and red arrows indicate positive associations).
Figure 3
Figure 3
Mean direct effects of variables on child BSID-III cognitive score at 24 months, comparing models with enteropathogens from non-diarrhoeal (green) or from diarrhoeal (pink) stool samples and including all observations (full) or the lower and upper tertiles of WAZ at enrolment. The significance in the figure indicates whether or not the specific variable was significant in the path model, with the horizontal axis (parameter estimate) providing an indication of the strength of association. BSID-III, Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development-III; WAZ, weight-for-age z-score.

References

    1. Gottfredson LS. Mainstream science on intelligence: an editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography. Intelligence 1997;24:13–23. 10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90011-8 - DOI
    1. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM. Show me the child at seven II: childhood intelligence and later outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2005;46:850–8. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01472.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Knickmeyer RC, Gouttard S, Kang C, et al. . A structural MRI study of human brain development from birth to 2 years. J Neurosci 2008;28:12176–82. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3479-08.2008 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Rose SA, Feldman JF, Futterweit LR, et al. . Continuity in tactual-visual cross-modal transfer: infancy to 11 years. Dev Psychol 1998;34:435–40. 10.1037/0012-1649.34.3.435 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Doyle LW, Davis PG, Schmidt B, et al. . Cognitive outcome at 24 months is more predictive than at 18 months for IQ at 8-9 years in extremely low birth weight children. Early Hum Dev 2012;88:95–8. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.07.013 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources