Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2018 Aug 14:7:e37241.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.37241.

Stimulus vignetting and orientation selectivity in human visual cortex

Affiliations

Stimulus vignetting and orientation selectivity in human visual cortex

Zvi N Roth et al. Elife. .

Abstract

Neural selectivity to orientation is one of the simplest and most thoroughly-studied cortical sensory features. Here, we show that a large body of research that purported to measure orientation tuning may have in fact been inadvertently measuring sensitivity to second-order changes in luminance, a phenomenon we term 'vignetting'. Using a computational model of neural responses in primary visual cortex (V1), we demonstrate the impact of vignetting on simulated V1 responses. We then used the model to generate a set of predictions, which we confirmed with functional MRI experiments in human observers. Our results demonstrate that stimulus vignetting can wholly determine the orientation selectivity of responses in visual cortex measured at a macroscopic scale, and suggest a reinterpretation of a well-established literature on orientation processing in visual cortex.

Keywords: fMRI; human; multivariate classification; neuroscience; orientation; primary visual cortex.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

ZR, DH, EM No competing interests declared

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.. Stimulus vignetting.
(A) Stimulus image (left) and Fourier amplitude (right) for sinusoidal grating that is infinite in extent. Black dots, Fourier amplitude restricted to a single spatial frequency and orientation component. Colored ovals, orientation tuning (polar angle cross-section through the oval) and spatial frequency tuning (radial cross-section) of two hypothetical V1 neurons. Red oval, neuron with broad orientation tuning and narrow spatial frequency tuning. Green oval, neuron with narrow orientation tuning and broad spatial frequency tuning. (B) Sinusoidal grating vignetted by vertical aperture. Light gray dots, spread of Fourier energy because of the aperture. (C) Sinusoidal grating vignetted by horizontal aperture.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.. Simulated neural responses illustrating the impact of stimulus vignetting.
Oriented gratings were vignetted by inner and outer annuli, using stimulus parameters identical to those from Freeman et al. (2011). Next, the gratings were used as input to the model. Model responses were computed separately for vertical (top) and horizontal (bottom) gratings. Finally, model output was computed as the responses to vertical minus responses to horizontal gratings. Model exhibits a preference for horizontal gratings along the horizontal meridian and a preference for vertical gratings along the vertical meridian (i.e. a radial bias).
Figure 3.
Figure 3.. Model predictions for modulated gratings.
Each of the eight stimuli were created by multiplying a vertical or horizontal grating by a radial or angular modulator. These stimuli were used as input to the model. Model responses were computed as in Figure 2. For radial modulated gratings (top two rows), the model exhibits a radial preference: larger responses to horizontal gratings along the horizontal meridian, larger responses to vertical gratings along the vertical meridian. However, for angular modulated gratings (bottom two rows), the orientation preference is tangential: larger responses for horizontal gratings along the vertical meridian and larger responses for vertical gratings along the horizontal meridian.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.. fMRI measurements of orientation bias depend on stimulus vignetting.
(A) Conventional resolution, 3T. Top: Responses to phase-encoded oriented gratings multiplied by a static radial modulator (shown in inset). The oriented grating cycled through 16 steps of orientation ranging from 0° to 180° every 24 s. The radial modulator was constant during the entire experiment. Map thresholded at coherence of 0.2. Hue indicates phase of the best fitting sinusoid. White lines indicate V1/V2 boundaries. Bottom: Responses to the same oriented grating as in A, but here the grating is multiplied by an angular modulator. As predicted by the model, the radial modulator gave rise to a radial orientation bias, while the angular modulator gave rise to a tangential orientation bias. (B) High-resolution, high field strength (7T) measurements of orientation preference for radial and angular modulators. Stimuli and conventions same as for A, except the modulators were radial and angular sinusoids.
Figure 4—figure supplement 1.
Figure 4—figure supplement 1.. fMRI responses for all individual subjects.
Left: responses to square wave modulators at 3T. Middle: responses to square wave modulators at 7T. Right: responses to sinusoidal modulators at 7T.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.. Orientation bias depends on the modulator.
Orientation bias for V1 voxels pooled across observers for the angular modulator (y-axis) and radial modulator (x-axis).
Figure 6.
Figure 6.. Combining the model with pRF estimates enables reliable predictions of single-voxel orientation preference.
(A) pRF sampling method. (1) Each voxel’s pRF was estimated based on an independent pRF-mapping scanning session. (2) Model output was sampled by the voxel pRF, resulting in an estimated preferred orientation for each modulator. (3) Estimated preferred orientation was compared to measured orientation preference, for each modulator. (B) Estimated orientation preference plotted against measured orientation preference. Sampling the model output with estimated pRFs yielded accurate predictions for each voxel’s orientation preference for each of the two modulators, indicating that the model of stimulus vignetting provides a good account for the fMRI measurements of orientation preference.
Figure 7.
Figure 7.. Population responses reflect the modulator, not stimulus orientation.
(A) fMRI decoding. Within-modulator decoding: classifier was trained and tested on data from each modulator separately, in a leave-one-run-out cross-validation approach. Across modulator decoding: classifier was trained on data from one modulator and tested on data from the other modulator (and vice-versa). Across-modulator decoding after shifting: data for one modulator was shifted by 90°, then the classifier was trained on that modulator and tested on the other (and vice-versa). (B) Multidimensional scaling. Top: population responses to all orientations for both modulators (solid lines, radial modulator; dotted lines, angular modulator) are plotted in 2D, such that distances between conditions reflect how different (i.e., dissimilarity = 1–r, where r is the correlation coefficient) the responses are. For the original data, orthogonal orientations from the two modulators have similar responses and are plotted near each other, while identical orientations taken from the two modulators evoked very different responses, and lay distant from each other. Bottom: after shifting the labels for the angular modulator by 90 deg, the two datasets align well, and similar orientations correspond to similar population responses regardless of the modulator.

Comment in

  • Framing orientation selectivity.
    de Lange FP, Ekman M. de Lange FP, et al. Elife. 2018 Aug 14;7:e39762. doi: 10.7554/eLife.39762. Elife. 2018. PMID: 30106374 Free PMC article.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adams DL, Sincich LC, Horton JC. Complete pattern of ocular dominance columns in human primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:10391–10403. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2923-07.2007. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Adelson EH, Bergen JR. Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. Journal of the Optical Society of America A. 1985;2:284–299. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.2.000284. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Alink A, Krugliak A, Walther A, Kriegeskorte N. fMRI orientation decoding in V1 does not require global maps or globally coherent orientation stimuli. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013;4:493. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00493. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Alink A, Walther A, Krugliak A, Kriegeskorte N. Local opposite orientation preferences in V1: fMRI sensitivity to fine-grained pattern information. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:7128. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-07036-8. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Andersson JL, Skare S, Ashburner J. How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: application to diffusion tensor imaging. NeuroImage. 2003;20:870–888. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00336-7. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources