Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2018 Aug 17;8(1):12364.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-30684-3.

FN400 amplitudes reveal the differentiation of semantic inferences within natural vs. artificial domains

Affiliations

FN400 amplitudes reveal the differentiation of semantic inferences within natural vs. artificial domains

Changquan Long et al. Sci Rep. .

Abstract

Category-based inferences allow inductions about novel properties based on categorical memberships (e.g., knowing all trout have genes [premise] allows us to infer that all fish have genes [conclusion]). Natural (N) and artificial (A) domains are the most obvious and traditional distinctions in categorization. The distinct event-related potential (ERP) responses for N and A domains have not yet been examined during category-based inferences. In this study, the differences between ERP inference parameters within N and A domains were measured during inductive decision processing, while controlling the premise-conclusion similarity and premise typicality between those two domains. Twenty-two adults were asked to make a decision on whether a conclusion was definitely weak, possibly weak, possibly strong, or definitely strong, based on a premise. The behavioral results showed that semantic inferences within the N domain shared similar inductive strength, similar "correct" response rates, and similar reaction times with that within the A domain. However, the ERP results showed that semantic inferences elicited smaller frontal-distributed N400 (FN400) amplitudes within the N domain than within the A domain, which suggested that knowledge of the ontological domain of a category affects category-based inferences, and underlaid the increased categorical coherence and homogeneity in the N as compared to the A categories. Therefore, we have distinguished the cognitive course of semantic inferences between N and A domains.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Experimental procedure.
Figure 2
Figure 2
The electrode layout and the region of interest (ROI). The red cluster is ROI that be used for the further two-factor repeated-measures analyses of variances (ANOVAs) for ERP data.
Figure 3
Figure 3
The averaged inference strength, “correct” response rates (ACC), and reaction times (RTs) of “correct” responses for each sub-condition. N+ indicates “congruent conclusions with a natural premise category”; A+ indicates “congruent conclusions with an artificial premise category”; N− indicates “incongruent conclusions with a natural premise category”; A− indicates “incongruent conclusions with an artificial premise category”. Error bars represent mean ± s.e.m. ***p < 0.001.
Figure 4
Figure 4
The ERP responses to the congruency effects. Figure 4a shows the grandaveraged waveforms elicited by N+ and N− arguments and the difference waveform (N− minus N+) in the fronto-central region, and the topography of the difference waveform at 250–400-ms. Figure 4b illustrates the grand-averaged waveforms elicited by A+ and A− arguments and the difference waveform (A− minus A+) in the fronto-central region, and the topography of the difference waveform at 250–400-ms. N+ indicates “congruent conclusions with a natural premise category”; A+ indicates “congruent conclusions with an artificial premise category”; N− indicates “incongruent conclusions with a natural premise category”; A− indicates “incongruent conclusions with an artificial premise category”.
Figure 5
Figure 5
The ERP responses to the domain effects. Figure 5a shows the grandaveraged waveforms elicited by N+ and A+ arguments and the difference waveform (A+ minus N+) in the fronto-central region, and the topography of the difference waveform at 250–400-ms. Figure 5b illustrates the grand-averaged waveforms elicited by N− and A− arguments and the difference waveform (A− minus N−) in the fronto-central region, and the topography of the difference waveform at 250–400-ms. N+ indicates “congruent conclusions with a natural premise category”; A+ indicates “congruent conclusions with an artificial premise category”; N− indicates “incongruent conclusions with a natural premise category”; A− indicates “incongruent conclusions with an artificial premise category”.

References

    1. Hawkins GE, Hayes BK, Heit E. A dynamic model of reasoning and memory. J. Exp. Psychol. –Gen. 2016;145:155–180. doi: 10.1037/xge0000113. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Markman AB, Ross BH. Category use and category learning. Psychol. Bull. 2003;129:592–613. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.592. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Kemp C, Jern A. A taxonomy of inductive problems. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2014;21:23–46. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0467-3. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Badger JR, Shapiro LR. Category structure affects the developmental trajectory of children’s inductive inferences for both natural kinds and artefacts. Think Reasoning. 2015;21:206–229. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2014.952338. - DOI
    1. Diesendruck G, Peretz S. Domain differences in the weights of perceptual and conceptual information in children’s categorization. Dev. Psychol. 2013;49:2383–2395. doi: 10.1037/a0032049. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

LinkOut - more resources