Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Comparative Study
. 2019 Jan;25(1):15-28.
doi: 10.1017/S135561771800070X. Epub 2018 Oct 18.

Effects of Bilingualism on Verbal and Nonverbal Memory Measures in Mild Cognitive Impairment

Affiliations
Comparative Study

Effects of Bilingualism on Verbal and Nonverbal Memory Measures in Mild Cognitive Impairment

Mónica Rosselli et al. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2019 Jan.

Abstract

Objectives: Maintaining two active languages may increase cognitive and brain reserve among bilingual individuals. We explored whether such a neuroprotective effect was manifested in the performance of memory tests for participants with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI).

Methods: We compared 42 bilinguals to 25 monolinguals on verbal and nonverbal memory tests. We used: (a) the Loewenstein-Acevedo Scales for Semantic Interference and Learning (LASSI-L), a sensitive test that taps into proactive, retroactive, and recovery from proactive semantic interference (verbal memory), and (b) the Benson Figure delayed recall (nonverbal memory). A subsample had volumetric MRI scans.

Results: The bilingual group significantly outperformed the monolingual group on two LASSI-L cued recall measures (Cued A2 and Cued B2). A measure of maximum learning (Cued A2) showed a correlation with the volume of the left hippocampus in the bilingual group only. Cued B2 recall (sensitive to recovery from proactive semantic interference) was correlated with the volume of the hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex of both cerebral hemispheres in the bilingual group, as well as with the left and right hippocampus in the monolingual group. The memory advantage in bilinguals on these measures was associated with higher inhibitory control as measured by the Stroop Color-Word test.

Conclusions: Our results demonstrated a superior performance of aMCI bilinguals over aMCI monolinguals on selected verbal memory tasks. This advantage was not observed in nonverbal memory. Superior memory performance of bilinguals over monolinguals suggests that bilinguals develop a different and perhaps more efficient semantic association system that influences verbal recall. (JINS, 2019, 25, 15-28).

Keywords: Alzheimer’s; Bilingualism; Hispanics/Latinos; MCI; Memory; Proactive interference; Spanish.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Sequence of the LASSIE-L Administration procedure

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Abutalebi J, & Green D (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(3), 242–275. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.10.003 - DOI
    1. Alladi S, Bak TH, Duggirala V, Surampudi B, Shailaja M, Shukla AK, … Kaul S (2013). Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81(22), 1938–1944. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000436620.33155.a4. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Antón-Méndez I, & Gollan TH (2010). Not just semantics: Strong frequency and weak cognate effects on semantic association in bilinguals. Memory & Cognition, 38(6), 723–739. doi:10.3758/MC.38.6.723 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ardila A (2007). Bilingualism in the contemporary world In Ardila A & Ramos E (Eds.), Speech and language disorders in bilinguals (pp.1–20). New York: Nova Science.
    1. Bak TH, Nissan JJ, Allerhand MM, & Deary IJ (2014). Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology, 75(6), 959–963. doi:10.1002/ana.24158. - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types