Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2018 Nov 6:9:2099.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02099. eCollection 2018.

Better, Not Just More-Contrast in Qualitative Aspects of Reward Facilitates Impulse Control in Pigs

Affiliations

Better, Not Just More-Contrast in Qualitative Aspects of Reward Facilitates Impulse Control in Pigs

Manuela Zebunke et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Delay-of-gratification paradigms, such as the famous "Marshmallow Test," are designed to investigate the complex cognitive concepts of self-control and impulse control in humans and animals. Such tests determine whether a subject will demonstrate impulse control by choosing a large, delayed reward over an immediate, but smaller reward. Documented relationships between impulsive behavior and aggression in humans and animals suggest important implications for farm animal husbandry and welfare, especially in terms of inadequate social behavior, tail biting and maternal behavior. In a preliminary study, we investigated whether the extent of impulse control would differ between quantitatively and qualitatively different aspects of reward in pigs. Twenty female piglets were randomly divided into two groups, with 10 piglets each. After a preference test to determine individual reward preference among six different food items, a discrimination test was conducted to train for successful discrimination between different amounts of reward (one piece vs. four pieces) and different qualitative aspects of reward (highly preferred vs. least preferred food item). Then, an increasing delay (2, 4, 8, 16, 24, 32 s) was introduced for the larger/highly preferred reward. Each piglet could choose to get the smaller/least preferred reward immediately or to wait for the larger/highly preferred reward. Piglets showed clear differences in their preference for food items. Moreover, the "quality group" displayed faster learning in the discrimination test (number of sessions until 90% of the animals completed the discrimination test: "quality group"-3 days vs. "quantity group"-5 days) and reached a higher level of impulse control in the delay-of-gratification test compared to the "quantity group" (maximum delay that was mastered: "quality group"-24 s vs. "quantity group"-8 s). These results demonstrate that impulse control is present in piglets but that the opportunity to get a highly preferred reward is more valued than the opportunity to get more of a given reward. This outcome also underlines the crucial role of motivation in cognitive test paradigms. Further investigations will examine whether impulse control is related to traits that are relevant to animal husbandry and welfare.

Keywords: delay choice task; delay of gratification; discrimination learning; impulsivity; motivation; pigs; preference test; reward learning.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Experimental pen with the experimental setup: grating with closable openings to the reward dishes fixed in a sliding board. The experimenter was positioned behind the grating and managed the openings and the sliding board, as well as baiting the reward dishes.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Percentage distribution of choices (N = 804) across the rewards used in the preference test for all animals in both groups.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Performance of the animals in both groups (quantitative difference in reward [amount: 1:4] vs. qualitative difference in reward [differentially preferred items: low: high]) during the discrimination test, i.e., the number of sessions needed to reach the learning criterion (significantly choosing the larger/highly preferred reward). The boxplot shows the distribution of the data from both groups with the 25, 50th (median), and 75th percentiles as gray boxes, the 90th percentile as a whisker and black circles as outliers.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Percentage distribution of the different behavioral strategies during decision-making across all trials of the animals in both groups (quantitative difference in reward [amount: 1:4] vs. qualitative difference in reward [differentially preferred items: low: high]) during the delay maintenance test. The boxplot shows the distribution of the data within the single strategies with the 25, 50th (median), and 75th percentiles as gray boxes, the 90th percentile as a whisker and black circles as outliers. The asterisk indicates a significant difference between both groups revealed with an unpaired t-test (* = P < 0.05).

References

    1. Addessi E., Bellagamba F., Delfino A., De Petrillo F., Focaroli V., Macchitella L., et al. . (2014). Waiting by mistake: symbolic representation of rewards modulates intertemporal choice in capuchin monkeys, preschool children and adult humans. Cognition 130, 428–441. 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.019 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Addessi E., Paglieri F., Beran M. J., Evans T. A., Macchitella L., De Petrillo F., et al. . (2013). Delay choice versus delay maintenance: different measures of delayed gratification in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J. Comp. Psychol. 127, 392–398. 10.1037/a0031869 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Addessi E., Rossi S. (2011). Tokens improve capuchin performance in the reverse-reward contingency task. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 278, 849–854. 10.1098/rspb.2010.1602 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Ainslie G. W. (1974). Impulse control in Pigeons. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 21, 485–489. 10.1901/jeab.1974.21-485 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Anderson J. R., Hattori Y., Fujita K. (2008). Quality before quantity: rapid learning of reverse-reward contingency by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J. Comp. Psychol. 122, 445–448. 10.1037/a0012624 - DOI - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources