Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019 Mar:179:143-161.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.10.009. Epub 2018 Dec 1.

Children judge others based on their food choices

Affiliations

Children judge others based on their food choices

Jasmine M DeJesus et al. J Exp Child Psychol. 2019 Mar.

Abstract

Individuals and cultures share some commonalities in food preferences, yet cuisines also differ widely across social groups. Eating is a highly social phenomenon; however, little is known about the judgments children make about other people's food choices. Do children view conventional food choices as normative and consequently negatively evaluate people who make unconventional food choices? In five experiments, 5-year-old children were shown people who ate conventional and unconventional foods, including typical food items paired in unconventional ways. In Experiment 1, children preferred conventional foods and conventional food eaters. Experiment 2 suggested a link between expectations of conventionality and native/foreign status; children in the United States thought that English speakers were relatively more likely to choose conventional foods than French speakers. Yet, children in Experiments 3 and 4 judged people who ate unconventional foods as negatively as they judged people who ate canonical disgust elicitors and nonfoods, even when considering people from a foreign culture. Children in Experiment 5 were more likely to assign conventional foods to cultural ingroup members than to cultural outgroup members; nonetheless, they thought that no one was likely to eat the nonconventional items. These results demonstrate that children make normative judgments about other people's food choices and negatively evaluate people across groups who deviate from conventional eating practices.

Keywords: Disgust; Food selection; Intergroup cognition; Social cognition; Social judgment; Social norms.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Example trials from Experiment 1 (conventional: milk with chocolate syrup, hot dog with mustard; unconventional: milk with mustard, hot dog with chocolate syrup), top, and Experiment 3 (conventional: watermelon; unconventional: hot dog with chocolate syrup; nonfood: grass; disgust: insect), bottom.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Children’s evaluations of others’ food choices and their social judgments in Experiments 1 (top), 3 (center), and 4 (bottom), by item type. Higher scores represent more yes/yummy/near responses (i.e., positive evaluations). Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Children’s evaluations of others’ food choices and their social judgments in Experiments 1 (top), 3 (center), and 4 (bottom), by item type. Higher scores represent more yes/yummy/near responses (i.e., positive evaluations). Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Example stimuli used to describe people as Cortanian (Experiments 4 and 5; top right) and American (Experiment 5; top left) and to display choice options in Experiment 5 (bottom).

References

    1. Aldridge V, Dovey TM, & Halford JCG (2009). The role of familiarity in dietary development. Developmental Review, 29(1), 32–44. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2008.11.001 - DOI
    1. Birch LL (1980). Effects of peer models’ food choices and eating behaviors on preschoolers’ food preferences. Child Development, 489–496. doi: 10.2307/1129283 - DOI
    1. Birch LL (1990). Development of food acceptance patterns. Developmental Psychology, 26(4), 515–519. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.26.4.515 - DOI
    1. Birch LL, & Marlin DW (1982). I don’t like it; I never tried it: Effects of exposure on two-year-old children’s food preferences. Appetite, 3(4), 353–360. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.26.4.515 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Cimpian A, & Salomon E (2014). The inherence heuristic: An intuitive means of making sense of the world, and a potential precursor to psychological essentialism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(5), 461–480. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13002197 - DOI - PubMed

Publication types