Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Randomized Controlled Trial
. 2019 Feb:116:14-19.
doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2018.12.012. Epub 2018 Dec 16.

Declines in motor transfer following upper extremity task-specific training in older adults

Affiliations
Randomized Controlled Trial

Declines in motor transfer following upper extremity task-specific training in older adults

Christopher S Walter et al. Exp Gerontol. 2019 Feb.

Abstract

Background: Age-related declines in function can limit older adults' independence with activities of daily living (ADLs). While task-specific training maybe a viable approach to improve function, limited clinical resources prevent extensive training on wide ranges of skills and contexts. Thus, training on one task for the benefit of another (i.e., transfer) is important in geriatric physical rehabilitation. The purpose of this study was to test whether motor transfer would occur between two functionally different upper extremity tasks that simulate ADLs in a sample of older adults following task-specific training.

Methods: Ninety community dwelling adults ages 43 to 94 years old performed two trials of a functional dexterity and functional reaching task at baseline, and were then assigned to one of two groups. The training group completed 3 days of task-specific training (150 trials) on the functional reaching task, whereas the no-training group received no training on either task. Both groups were re-tested on both tasks at the end of Day 3.

Results: No significant interactions were observed between group (training vs. no-training) and time (baseline vs. re-test) on the functional dexterity task (i.e. transfer task), indicating no difference in the average amount of change from baseline to re-test between the groups. However, post hoc bivariate linear regression revealed an effect of age on motor transfer within the training group. For those who trained on the functional reaching task, the amount of transfer to the dexterity task was inversely related to age. There was no significant relationship between age and motor transfer for the no-training group.

Discussion and conclusions: Results of our a priori group analysis suggest that functional reaching training did not, on average, transfer to the dexterity task. However, post hoc regression analysis showed that motor transfer was both experience- and age-dependent, such that motor transfer may decline with advanced age. Future research will consider how functional and cognitive aging influences transfer of motor skills across different activities of daily living.

Keywords: Activities of daily living (ADLs); Aging; Functional training; Generalization; Motor learning; Rehabilitation.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

Conflicts of Interest: The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Overhead view of (A) dexterity task and (B) reaching task. Note that the dexterity task served as the transfer task in this study.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
(A) Mean trial time on the functional reaching task over the 150 training trials. Error bars indicate standard error. Data are only shown for the training group (no-training group did not train on this task). (B) Mean trial time on the functional reaching task at baseline and re-test for the training (gray) and no-training (black) groups. Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Mean trial time on the functional dexterity task at baseline and re-test for the training (gray) and no-training (black) groups. Error bars indicate standard error.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Percent transfer plotted as a function of participant age for the training group. Data shown above are for participants in the training group (r=−.42; p=.0064). No data are shown for the no-training group due to no significant age effect.

References

    1. Andrews AW, Thomas MW, & Bohannon RW (1996). Normative values for isometric muscle force measurements obtained with hand-held dynamometers. Phys Ther, 76(3), 248–259. - PubMed
    1. Arya KN, Verma R, Garg RK, Sharma VP, Agarwal M, & Aggarwal GG (2012). Meaningful task-specific training (MTST) for stroke rehabilitation: a randomized controlled trial. Top Stroke Rehabil, 19(3), 193–211. doi:10.1310/tsr1903-193 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Backman C, Mackie H, & Harris J (1991). Arthritis hand function test: Development of a standardized assessment tool. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 11, 245–256.
    1. Berghuis KMM, Semmler JG, Opie GM, Post AK, & Hortobágyi T (2017). Age-related changes in corticospinal excitability and intracortical inhibition after upper extremity motor learning: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurobiol Aging, 55, 61–71. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.03.024 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bier B, Ouellet É, & Belleville S (2018). Computerized attentional training and transfer with virtual reality: Effect of age and training type. Neuropsychology, 32(5), 597–614. doi:10.1037/neu0000417 - DOI - PubMed

Publication types