Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019 Jan 16;14(1):e0210267.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210267. eCollection 2019.

Rapid detection of internalizing diagnosis in young children enabled by wearable sensors and machine learning

Affiliations

Rapid detection of internalizing diagnosis in young children enabled by wearable sensors and machine learning

Ryan S McGinnis et al. PLoS One. .

Abstract

There is a critical need for fast, inexpensive, objective, and accurate screening tools for childhood psychopathology. Perhaps most compelling is in the case of internalizing disorders, like anxiety and depression, where unobservable symptoms cause children to go unassessed-suffering in silence because they never exhibiting the disruptive behaviors that would lead to a referral for diagnostic assessment. If left untreated these disorders are associated with long-term negative outcomes including substance abuse and increased risk for suicide. This paper presents a new approach for identifying children with internalizing disorders using an instrumented 90-second mood induction task. Participant motion during the task is monitored using a commercially available wearable sensor. We show that machine learning can be used to differentiate children with an internalizing diagnosis from controls with 81% accuracy (67% sensitivity, 88% specificity). We provide a detailed description of the modeling methodology used to arrive at these results and explore further the predictive ability of each temporal phase of the mood induction task. Kinematical measures most discriminative of internalizing diagnosis are analyzed in detail, showing affected children exhibit significantly more avoidance of ambiguous threat. Performance of the proposed approach is compared to clinical thresholds on parent-reported child symptoms which differentiate children with an internalizing diagnosis from controls with slightly lower accuracy (.68-.75 vs. .81), slightly higher specificity (.88-1.00 vs. .88), and lower sensitivity (.00-.42 vs. .67) than the proposed, instrumented method. These results point toward the future use of this approach for screening children for internalizing disorders so that interventions can be deployed when they have the highest chance for long-term success.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves for models trained to detect children with internalizing diagnoses.
Curves for logistic regressions trained on data from the potential threat, startle, and response modulation phases of the snake task are indicated in blue, red, and yellow, respectively. The model trained on data from the potential threat phase performs better than the other models.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Boxplots of error rates for models trained to detect children with internalizing diagnoses compared to those due to chance for each temporal phase of the snake task.
Error rates due to random chance determined via permutation test are shown in gray, while those from the actual data are in teal. Statistically significant differences are noted with an asterisk. The model trained on data from the potential threat phase is the only one to outperform random chance.
Fig 3
Fig 3
Yaw angle time series from selected subjects (a) and boxplots of selected features from all subjects (b). Time series data from a subject with an internalizing diagnosis is shown in gray, while that from a subject without is shown in teal. Similarly, gray boxplots correspond to data from subjects with a diagnosis while teal boxplots are from those without. The significant deviation in the yaw angle between subjects noted in (a) is reflected across all subjects in the boxplots of (b).

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Egger HL, Angold A. Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47: 313–337. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01618.x - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bufferd SJ, Dougherty LR, Carlson GA, Klein DN. Parent-Reported Mental Health in Preschoolers: Findings Using a Diagnostic Interview. Compr Psychiatry. 2011;52: 359–369. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.08.006 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Luby JL, Si X, Belden AC, Tandon M, Spitznagel E. Preschool depression: Homotypic continuity and course over 24 months. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66: 897–905. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.97 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Tandon M, Cardeli E, Luby J. Internalizing Disorders in Early Childhood: A Review of Depressive and Anxiety Disorders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2009;18: 593–610. 10.1016/j.chc.2009.03.004 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Luby JL, Belden AC, Pautsch J, Si X, Spitznagel E. The clinical significance of preschool depression: Impairment in functioning and clinical markers of the disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2009;112: 111–119. 10.1016/j.jad.2008.03.026 - DOI - PMC - PubMed

Publication types