Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019 Jan 15:9:2598.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02598. eCollection 2018.

Examining the Effects of Couples' Real-Time Stress and Coping Processes on Interaction Quality: Language Use as a Mediator

Affiliations

Examining the Effects of Couples' Real-Time Stress and Coping Processes on Interaction Quality: Language Use as a Mediator

Kevin K H Lau et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Stress in romantic relationships is an all-too-common phenomenon that has detrimental effects on relationship well-being. Specifically, stress can lead to negative interactions between partners and ultimately decrease relationship functioning. The systemic-transactional model of dyadic coping posits that by effectively communicating stress and coping with one's romantic partner, couples can mitigate the deleterious effects of stress. Specifically, partners can engage in positive dyadic coping, which may foster couples' sense of "we-ness," strengthen their emotional connection, and facilitate their understanding of each other's stressful experiences. However, these associations have not yet been examined during partners' real-time stress conversations. When assessing dyadic coping, a particular aspect of interest is partners' language use (i.e., pronouns, emotion words, and cognition words), as it may reflect the types of support they communicate to one another. Using real-time interaction data from 41 heterosexual couples, this study examined how couples' stress and coping processes affect perceived interaction quality following discussions of stress. Specifically, language use (i.e., pronouns, emotion words, and cognition words) was assessed as a mediator on the association between observed stress communication and perceived interaction quality. Overall, results supported our hypotheses; when one partner communicated stress, the other partner responded with language use indicative of different types of dyadic coping (i.e., more you-talk and use of emotion words, less we-talk, I-talk, and use of cognition words), which were in turn associated with interaction quality in mixed directions. Implications of these findings for romantic couples are discussed.

Keywords: interaction quality; language use; real-time interaction data; romantic relationships; stress; systemic-transactional model.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Proposed model of language use (Time 1) mediating the association between stress communication (Time 1) and interaction quality (Time 2).
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Unstandardized model results of we-talk (red), I-talk (orange), and you-talk (yellow) mediating the association between general stress communication and perceived interaction quality. Only one set of coefficients were included due to the indistinguishability of partner roles. p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Unstandardized model results of positive (green) and negative emotion words (blue) mediating the association between emotion-focused stress communication and perceived interaction quality. Only one set of coefficients were included due to the indistinguishability of partner roles. p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001.
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Unstandardized model results of cognitive processing words mediating the association between problem-focused stress communication and perceived interaction quality. Only one set of coefficients were included due to the indistinguishability of partner roles. p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Amato P. R., Previti D. (2003). People’s reasons for divorcing: gender, social class, the life course, and adjustment. J. Fam. Issues 24 602–626. 10.1177/0192513X03024005002 - DOI
    1. Aron A., Norman C. C., Aron E. N., McKenna C., Heyman R. E. (2000). Couples’ shared participation in novel and arousing activities and experienced relationship quality. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78 273–284. 10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.273 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Baddeley J. L., Pennebaker J. W. (2011). A postdeployment expressive writing intervention for military couples: a randomized controlled trial. J. Trauma Stress 24 581–585. 10.1002/jts.20679 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bahr S. J. (1979). The effects of welfare on marital stability and remarriage. J. Marriage Fam. 41 553–560. 10.2307/351625 - DOI
    1. Biesen J. N., Schooler D., Smith D. (2015). What a difference a pronoun makes: I/We versus you/me and worried couples’ perceptions of their interaction quality. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 35 1–26.

LinkOut - more resources