Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2019 Jan 17:9:2707.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02707. eCollection 2018.

Avian Emotions: Comparative Perspectives on Fear and Frustration

Affiliations
Review

Avian Emotions: Comparative Perspectives on Fear and Frustration

Mauricio R Papini et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

Emotions are complex reactions that allow individuals to cope with significant positive and negative events. Research on emotion was pioneered by Darwin's work on emotional expressions in humans and animals. But Darwin was concerned mainly with facial and bodily expressions of significance for humans, citing mainly examples from mammals (e.g., apes, dogs, and cats). In birds, emotional expressions are less evident for a human observer, so a different approach is needed. Understanding avian emotions will provide key evolutionary information on the evolution of related behaviors and brain circuitry. Birds and mammals are thought to have evolved from different groups of Mesozoic reptiles, theropod dinosaurs and therapsids, respectively, and therefore, their common ancestor is likely to be a basal reptile living about 300 million years ago, during the Carboniferous or Permian period. Yet, birds and mammals exhibit extensive convergence in terms of relative brain size, high levels of activity, sleep/wakefulness cycles, endothermy, and social behavior, among others. This article focuses on two basic emotions with negative valence: fear and frustration. Fear is related to the anticipation of dangerous or threatening stimuli (e.g., predators or aggressive conspecifics). Frustration is related to unexpected reward omissions or devaluations (e.g., loss of food or sexual resources). These results have implications for an understanding of the conditions that promote fear and frustration and for the evolution of supporting brain circuitry.

Keywords: aggression; birds; comparative psychology; conflict; emotion; fear; frustration; response suppression.

PubMed Disclaimer

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Abate G., Colazingari S., Accoto A., Conversi D., Bevilacqua A. (2018). Dendritic spine density and EphrinB2 levels of hippocampal and anterior cingulate cortex neurons increase sequentially during formation of recent and remote fear memory in the mouse. Behav. Brain Res. 344, 120–131. 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.02.011, PMID: - DOI - PubMed
    1. Ainslie G. W. (1974). Impulse control in pigeons. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 21, 485–489. 10.1901/jeab.1974.21-485, PMID: - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Alonso P. D., Milner A. C., Ketcham R. A., Cookson M. J., Rowe T. B. (2004). The avian nature of the brain and inner ear of Archaeopteryx. Nature 430, 666–669. 10.1038/nature02706, PMID: - DOI - PubMed
    1. Amsel A. (1992). Frustration theory. An analysis of dispositional learning and memory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 10.1037/0033-2909.112.3.396, PMID: - DOI - PubMed
    1. Amsel A., Roussel J. (1952). Motivational properties of frustration: I. Effect on a running response of the addition of frustration to the motivational complex. J. Exp. Psychol. 43, 363–368. 10.1037/h0059393, PMID: - DOI - PubMed