Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019 Jan 30:9:2757.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02757. eCollection 2018.

What Machine Learning Can Tell Us About the Role of Language Dominance in the Diagnostic Accuracy of German LITMUS Non-word and Sentence Repetition Tasks

Affiliations

What Machine Learning Can Tell Us About the Role of Language Dominance in the Diagnostic Accuracy of German LITMUS Non-word and Sentence Repetition Tasks

Lina Abed Ibrahim et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

The present study investigates the performance of 21 monolingual and 56 bilingual children aged 5;6-9;0 on German LITMUS-sentence-repetition (SRT; Hamann et al., 2013) and non-word-repetition-tasks (NWRT; Grimm et al., 2014), which were constructed in accordance with the LITMUS-principles (Language Impairment Testing in Multilingual Settings; Armon-Lotem et al., 2015). Both tasks incorporate phonologically and syntactically complex structures shown to be cross-linguistically challenging for children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI) and aim at minimizing bias against bilingual children while still being indicative of the presence of language impairment across language combinations (see Marinis and Armon-Lotem, 2015; for sentence-repetition; Chiat, 2015 for non-word-repetition). Given the great variability in bilingual language exposure and the potential effect of language experience on language performance in bilingual children, we examined whether background variables related to bilingualism, particularly, the degree language dominance as measured by relative amount of use and exposure, could compromise the diagnostic accuracy of the German LITMUS-SRT and NWRT. We further investigated whether a combination of the two tasks provides better diagnostic accuracy and helps avoid cases of misdiagnosis. To address this, we used an unsupervised machine learning algorithm, the Partitioning-Around-Medoids (PAM, Kaufman and Rousseeuw, 2009), for deriving a clinical category for the children as ± language-impaired based on their performance scores on SRT and NWRT (in isolation and combined) while withholding information about their clinical status based on standardized assessment in their first (home language, L1) and second language (societal language, L2). Subsequently, we calculated diagnostic accuracy and used regression analysis to investigate which background variables (age of onset, length of exposure, degree of language dominance, socio-economic-status, and risk factors for SLI) best explained clinical-group-membership yielded from the PAM-analysis based on the children's NWRT and SRT performance scores. Results show that although language-dominance clearly influences the performance of bilingual typically developing children, especially in the SRT, the diagnostic accuracy of the tools is not compromised by language dominance: while risk factors for SLI were significant predictors for clinical group membership in all models, language dominance did not contribute at all to explaining clinical cluster membership as typically developing or SLI based on any of the combinations of the SRT and NWRT variables. Additionally, results confirm that a combination of SRT scored by correct target structure and the structurally more complex language-dependent part of the NWRT yields better diagnostic accuracy than single measures and is only sensitive to risk factors for SLI and not to dominance levels or SES.

Keywords: bilingualism; conditional inference trees; k-medoid clustering algorithm; language dominance; non-word repetition; sentence repetition; specific language impairment; unsupervised learning.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
PaBiQ: Calculation of language exposure index.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
NWRT: % correct identical repetition (NWRT_global, NWRT_LI, and NWRT_LD).
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
SRT: % identical repetition (SRT_Id) and correct production of target structure (SRT_Tar).
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
PAM-clustering of MoSLI+MoTD based on performance scores on SRT_Id, SRT_Tar, NWRT_global, NWRT_LI, and NWRT_LD.
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
PAM-clustering of MoSLI+BiTD based on performance scores on SRT_Id, SRT_Tar, NWRT_global, NWRT_LI, and NWRT_LD.
FIGURE 6
FIGURE 6
Conditional Inference Tree Analysis of the MoSLI+BiTD clustering result using SRT_Id, SRT_Tar, NWRT_global, NWRT_LI, and NWRT_LD as predictors.
FIGURE 7
FIGURE 7
Conditional Inference Tree Analysis of the BiSLI+BiTD clustering result based on SRT_Id and SRT_Tar: hierarchical variable importance of background variables predicting cluster membership.
FIGURE 8
FIGURE 8
PAM-clustering of BiSLI+BiTD based on performance scores on SRT_Id, SRT_Tar, NWRT_global, NWRT_LI, and NWRT_LD.
FIGURE 9
FIGURE 9
Conditional Inference Tree Analysis of the BiSLI+BiTD clustering result using SRT_Id, SRT_Tar, NWRT_global, NWRT_LI, and NWRT_LD as predictors.
FIGURE 10
FIGURE 10
PAM-clustering of BiSLI+BiTD using performance scores on SRT_Tar and NWRT_LD.

References

    1. Abed Ibrahim L., Hamann C. (2017). “Bilingual Arabic-German & Turkish-German children with and without specific language impairment: comparing performance in sentence and Nonword repetition tasks,” in Proceedings of BUCLD 41, eds LaMendola M., Scott J. (Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press; ), 1–17.
    1. Abed Ibrahim L., Hamann C., Öwerdieck D. (2018). “Identifying specific language impairment (SLI) across different bilingual populations: German sentence repetition task (SRT),” in Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, eds Bertolini A. B., Kaplan M. J. (Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press; ), 1–14.
    1. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association [ASHA] (2004). Knowledge and Skills Needed by Speech- Language Pathologists and Audiologists to Provide Culturally and Linguistically Appropriate Services [Knowledge and Skills]. Available at: www.asha.org/policy
    1. Archibald L. M., Gathercole S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition: a comparison of tests. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 49 970–983. 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/070) - DOI - PubMed
    1. Archibald L. M., Gathercole S. E. (2007). The complexities of complex memory span: storage and processing deficits in specific language impairment. J. Mem. Lang. 57 177–194. 10.1016/j.jml.2006.11.004 - DOI