Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2020 Jul;57(7):e13346.
doi: 10.1111/psyp.13346. Epub 2019 Feb 22.

P300 development from infancy to adolescence

Affiliations
Review

P300 development from infancy to adolescence

Tracy Riggins et al. Psychophysiology. 2020 Jul.

Abstract

This article provides an overview of P300 research from infancy through adolescence. First, a brief historical overview is provided highlighting seminal studies that began exploration of the P300 component in developmental groups. Overall, these studies suggest that the P300 can be detected in children and appears to reflect similar cognitive processes to those in adults; however, it is significantly delayed in its latency to peak. Second, two striking findings from developmental research are the lack of a clear P300 component in infancy and differential electrophysiological responses to novel, unexpected stimuli in children, adolescents, and adults. Third, contemporary questions are described, which include P300-like components in infancy, alteration of P300 in atypically developing groups, relations between P300 and behavior, individual differences of P300, and neural substrates of P300 across development. Finally, we conclude with comments regarding the power of a developmental perspective and suggestions for important issues that should be addressed in the next 50 years of P300 research.

Keywords: P300; P3a; P3b; adolescents; children; development; infants.

PubMed Disclaimer

References

REFERENCES

    1. Ackles, P. K., & Cook, K. G. (1998). Stimulus probability and event related potentials of the brain in 6-month-old human infants: A parametric study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 29(2), 115-143. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(98)00013-0
    1. Ackles, P. K., & Cook, K. G. (2009). Event related brain potentials and visual attention in six-month-old infants. International Journal of Neuroscience, 119(9), 1446-1468. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207450802330579
    1. Anokhin, A. P., Golosheykin, S., & Heath, A. C. (2010). Heritability of individual differences in cortical processing of facial affect. Behavior Genetics, 40(2), 178-185. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-010-9337-1
    1. Barriga-Paulino, C. I., Benjumea, Á. R., Rodríguez-Martínez, E. I., & Gómez González, C. M. (2015). Fronto-temporo-occipital activity changes with age during a visual working memory developmental study in children, adolescents and adults. Neuroscience Letters, 559, 26-31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2015.05.017
    1. Barry-Anwar, R., Hadley, H., & Scott, L. S. (2018). Differential neural responses to faces paired with labels versus faces paired with noise. Vision Research. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2018.03.002