Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019;16(2):137-151.
doi: 10.1080/17405629.2017.1356713. Epub 2017 Jul 24.

Cross-Cultural Differences in Temperament: Comparing Paternal ratings of US and Dutch infants

Affiliations

Cross-Cultural Differences in Temperament: Comparing Paternal ratings of US and Dutch infants

Eric Desmarais et al. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2019.

Abstract

This study conducted longitudinal comparisons of US and Dutch paternal ratings of temperament, measured via the Infant Behaviour Questionnaire-Revised, at 4 months (US n= 99; Dutch n= 127) and 12 months (US n= 66; Dutch n= 112) of age. US fathers rated their infant higher in the broad temperament trait Surgency, and its subscales vocal reactivity, high-intensity pleasure, and activity level. US fathers also rated their infants higher in Negative Emotionality, and its subscales of sadness, distress to limitations, and fear. Dutch infants received higher ratings in falling reactivity. Though the cultures did not differ in ratings of Orienting/regulatory capacity, US infants were higher on the subscale duration of orienting, and lower in soothability. Significant culture-by-age and culture-by-gender interactions were also noted. Overall, results are largely consistent with those reported for Dutch mothers (Sung et al., 2014) and speak to considerable differences in early temperament development between cultures viewed as largely similar because of their Western/individualistic orientations.

Keywords: Fathers; cross-cultural research; infant temperament.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1:
Figure 1:
Cross-cultural comparison of factor and subscale means. Note:Significant differences (p<.05) are indicated by *.

References

    1. Bayly B, & Gartstein MA (2013). Mother’s and father’s reports on their child’s temperament: Does gender matter? Infant Behavior and Development, 36(1), 171–175. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bernier A, Carlson SM, & Whipple N (2010). From External Regulation to Self-Regulation : Early Parenting Precursors of Young Children’s Executive Functioning. Child Development. 81(1), 326–339. - PubMed
    1. Bögels S, Stevens J, & Majdandžić M (2011). Parenting and social anxiety: Fathers’ versus mothers’ influence on their children’s anxiety in ambiguous social situations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 52(5), 599–606. - PubMed
    1. Bornstein MH (2013). Parenting and child mental health: a cross-cultural perspective. World Psychiatry : Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA), 12(3), 258–65. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bornstein MH, Haynes OM, Pascual L, Painter KM, & Galperín C (1999). Play in two societies: pervasiveness of process, specificity of structure. Child Development, 70(2), 317–331. - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources