Objecting to experiments that compare two unobjectionable policies or treatments
- PMID: 31072934
- PMCID: PMC6561206
- DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1820701116
Objecting to experiments that compare two unobjectionable policies or treatments
Abstract
Randomized experiments have enormous potential to improve human welfare in many domains, including healthcare, education, finance, and public policy. However, such "A/B tests" are often criticized on ethical grounds even as similar, untested interventions are implemented without objection. We find robust evidence across 16 studies of 5,873 participants from three diverse populations spanning nine domains-from healthcare to autonomous vehicle design to poverty reduction-that people frequently rate A/B tests designed to establish the comparative effectiveness of two policies or treatments as inappropriate even when universally implementing either A or B, untested, is seen as appropriate. This "A/B effect" is as strong among those with higher educational attainment and science literacy and among relevant professionals. It persists even when there is no reason to prefer A to B and even when recipients are treated unequally and randomly in all conditions (A, B, and A/B). Several remaining explanations for the effect-a belief that consent is required to impose a policy on half of a population but not on the entire population; an aversion to controlled but not to uncontrolled experiments; and a proxy form of the illusion of knowledge (according to which randomized evaluations are unnecessary because experts already do or should know "what works")-appear to contribute to the effect, but none dominates or fully accounts for it. We conclude that rigorously evaluating policies or treatments via pragmatic randomized trials may provoke greater objection than simply implementing those same policies or treatments untested.
Keywords: A/B tests; field experiments; pragmatic trials; randomized controlled trials; research ethics.
Copyright © 2019 the Author(s). Published by PNAS.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Figures



Comment in
-
The minimum mean paradox: A mechanical explanation for apparent experiment aversion.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019 Nov 26;116(48):23883-23884. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1912413116. Epub 2019 Nov 6. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019. PMID: 31694881 Free PMC article. No abstract available.
-
Reply to Mislavsky et al.: Sometimes people really are averse to experiments.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019 Nov 26;116(48):23885-23886. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1914509116. Epub 2019 Nov 12. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019. PMID: 31719207 Free PMC article. No abstract available.
References
-
- Baldassarri D, Abascal M (2017) Field experiments across the social sciences. Annu Rev Sociol 43:41–73.
-
- Haynes L, Service O, Goldacre B, Torgerson D (2012) Test, learn, adapt: Developing public policy with randomised controlled trials (Cabinet Office Behavioural Insights Team, London). Available at https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploa.... Accessed December 8, 2018.
-
- Greiner DJ, Pattanayak CW (2012) Randomized evaluation in legal assistance: What difference does representation (offer and actual use) make? Yale Law J 121:2118–2214.
-
- Institute of Medicine Roundtable on Evidence-Based Medicine (2007) The Learning Healthcare System: Workshop Summary, eds Olsen L, Aisner D, McGinnis JM. Olsen L, Aisner D, McGinnis JM (National Academies Press, Washington, DC: ). - PubMed
-
- Connolly P, Biggart A, Miller S, O’Hare L, Thurston A (2017) Using Randomised Controlled Trials in Education (Sage, London: ).
MeSH terms
LinkOut - more resources
Full Text Sources