Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2020 Dec;20(8):1495-1501.
doi: 10.1037/emo0000610. Epub 2019 Jun 13.

The role of imagery in threat-related perceptual decision making

Affiliations

The role of imagery in threat-related perceptual decision making

Gabriella Imbriano et al. Emotion. 2020 Dec.

Abstract

Visual perception is heavily influenced by "top-down" factors, including goals, expectations, and prior knowledge about the environmental context. Recent research has demonstrated the beneficial role threat-related cues play in perceptual decision making; however, the psychological processes contributing to this differential effect remain unclear. Since visual imagery helps to create perceptual representations or "templates" based on prior knowledge (e.g., cues), the present study examines the role vividness of visual imagery plays in enhanced perceptual decision making following threatening cues. In a perceptual decision-making task, participants used threat-related and neutral cues to detect perceptually degraded fearful and neutral faces presented at predetermined perceptual thresholds. Participants' vividness of imagery was measured by the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire-2 (VVIQ-2). Our results replicated prior work demonstrating that threat cues improve accuracy, perceptual sensitivity, and speed of perceptual decision making compared to neutral cues. Furthermore, better performance following threat and neutral cues was associated with higher VVIQ-2 scores. Importantly, more precise and rapid perceptual decision making following threatening cues was associated with greater VVIQ-2 scores, even after controlling for performance related to neutral cues. This association may be because greater imagery ability allows one to conjure more vivid threat-related templates, which facilitate subsequent perception. While the detection of threatening stimuli is well studied in the literature, our findings elucidate how threatening cues occurring prior to the stimulus aid in subsequent perception. Overall, these findings highlight the necessity of considering top-down threat-related factors in visual perceptual decision making. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Upper panel: A. Timeline of the threshold task. Perceptual thresholds, 75% correct, were found for fearful and neutral faces. B. Adaptive staircases, which made images harder or easier to see based on subject responses, were used in the threshold task to find each participant’s threshold for fearful and neutral faces. C. Cue and stimulus pairs used in the cued task: fear cue/fearful face (FC/FF), neutral cue/fearful face (NC/FF), fear cue/neutral face (FC/NF), and neutral cue/neutral face (NC/NF). D. Timeline of cue task. Participants used cues to respond to a perceptually degraded fearful or neutral face. Lower Panel: Compared to neutral cues, threatening cues led to improvement in E. d-Prime, F. accuracy, and G. Reaction time Faces used in this figure are from the NimStim Face Stimulus Set (Tottenham et al., 2009), a publically available set of emotional face stimuli. The models pictured above have consented to having images of their faces published in scientific journals.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Scatterplots displaying correlations between vividness of imagery (VVIQ-2) scores and behavioral performance measures showing greater VVIQ-2 was associated with A. increased accuracy for fear cues (FC),B. increased accuracy for neutral cues (NC), C. improved d-prime for FC, D. improved d-prime for NC, E. faster reaction time (RT) for FC, F. a trending but nonsignificant negative relationship with RT for NC.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Adini Y, Wilkonsky A, Haspel R, Tsodyks M, & Sagi D (2004). Perceptual learning in contrast discrimination: The effect of contrast uncertainty. Journal of Vision, 4(12), 2 10.1167/4.12.2 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Åhs F, Miller SS, Gordon AR, & Lundström JN (2013). Aversive learning increases sensory detection sensitivity. Biological Psychology, 92, 135–141. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.11.004 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Albright TD (2012). On the perception of probable things: Neural substrates of associative memory, imagery, and perception. Neuron, 74, 227–245. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.001 - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bywaters M, Andrade J, & Turpin G (2004). Determinants of the vividness of visual imagery: The effects of delayed recall, stimulus affect and individual differences. Memory, 12, 479–488. 10.1080/09658210444000160 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Campos A (2011). Internal consistency and construct validity of two versions of the Revised Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 113, 454–460. 10.2466/04.22.PMS.113.5.454-460 - DOI - PubMed