Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019 Jun 20;14(6):e0218550.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218550. eCollection 2019.

Training of paced breathing at 0.1 Hz improves CO2 homeostasis and relaxation during a paced breathing task

Affiliations

Training of paced breathing at 0.1 Hz improves CO2 homeostasis and relaxation during a paced breathing task

Mikołaj Tytus Szulczewski. PLoS One. .

Abstract

Volitional control of breathing often leads to excessive ventilation (hyperventilation) among untrained individuals, which disrupts CO2 homeostasis and may elicit a set of undesirable symptoms. The present study investigated whether seven days of training without any anti-hyperventilation instructions improves CO2 homeostasis during paced breathing at a frequency of 0.1 Hz (6 breaths/minute). Furthermore, the present study investigated the effects of training on breathing-related changes in affective state to examine the hypothesis that training improves the influence of slow paced breathing on affect. A total of 16 participants performed ten minutes of paced breathing every day for seven days. Partial pressure of end-tidal CO2 (PetCO2), symptoms of hyperventilation, affective state (before and after breathing), and pleasantness of the task were measured on the first, fourth, and seventh days of training. Results showed that the drop in PetCO2 significantly decreased with training and none of the participants experienced a drop in PetCO2 below 30 mmHg by day seven of training (except one participant who already had PetCO2 below 30 mmHg during baseline), in comparison to 37.5% of participants on the first day. Paced breathing produced hyperventilation symptoms of mild intensity which did not decrease with training. This suggests that some participants still experienced a drop of PetCO2 that was deep enough to produce noticeable symptoms. Affective state was shifted towards calmness and relaxation during the second and third laboratory measurements, but not during the first measurement. Additionally, the breathing task was perceived as more pleasant during subsequent laboratory measurements. The obtained results showed that training paced breathing at 0.1 Hz led to decrease in hyperventilation. Furthermore, the present study suggests that training paced breathing is necessary to make the task more pleasant and relaxing.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1
Fig 1. Changes in drop in PetCO2 between the three laboratory measurements with 95% confidence intervals.
Changes computed by subtraction of PetCO2 during paced breathing from PetCO2 during baseline. Note. * p < .05 *** p < .001; the significance of the difference between the laboratory measurements is indicated.
Fig 2
Fig 2. Changes in the intensity of symptoms of hyperventilation between the three laboratory measurements with 95% confidence intervals.
Changes computed by subtraction of symptom intensity before paced breathing from symptoms experienced during paced breathing.
Fig 3
Fig 3. Changes in the perceived pleasantness of the paced breathing task between the three laboratory measurements with 95% confidence intervals.
Note. ** p < .01; the significance of the difference between the laboratory measurements is indicated.
Fig 4
Fig 4. Changes in decrease in unpleasant arousal between the three laboratory measurements with 95% confidence intervals.
Changes computed by subtraction of unpleasant arousal after paced breathing from unpleasant arousal during baseline. Note. *** p < .001; the significance of the difference between the laboratory measurements is indicated.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Lehrer PM, Vaschillo E, Vaschillo B. Resonant frequency biofeedback training to increase cardiac variability: Rationale and manual for training. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2000;25(3):177–91. 10.1023/a:1009554825745 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Vaschillo EG, Vaschillo B, Lehrer PM. Characteristics of Resonance in Heart Rate Variability Stimulated by Biofeedback. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. Springer Nature; 2006. June;31(2):129–42. 10.1007/s10484-006-9009-3 - DOI - PubMed
    1. Wheat AL, Larkin KT. Biofeedback of Heart Rate Variability and Related Physiology: A Critical Review. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2010. May 5;35(3):229–42. 10.1007/s10484-010-9133-y - DOI - PubMed
    1. Karavidas MK, Lehrer PM, Vaschillo E, Vaschillo B, Marin H, Buyske S, et al. Preliminary Results of an Open Label Study of Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback for the Treatment of Major Depression. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2007. March 1;32(1):19–30. 10.1007/s10484-006-9029-z - DOI - PubMed
    1. Siepmann M, Aykac V, Unterdörfer J, Petrowski K, Mueck-Weymann M. A Pilot Study on the Effects of Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback in Patients with Depression and in Healthy Subjects. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback; 2008. September 19;33(4):195–201. 10.1007/s10484-008-9064-z - DOI - PubMed

Publication types