Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019 Dec;29(12):1980-1987.
doi: 10.1111/sms.13527. Epub 2019 Aug 21.

Investigating the construct of motor competence in middle childhood using the BOT-2 Short Form: An item response theory perspective

Affiliations
Free article

Investigating the construct of motor competence in middle childhood using the BOT-2 Short Form: An item response theory perspective

Farid Bardid et al. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2019 Dec.
Free article

Abstract

Purpose: Motor assessments generally produce a single motor competence score based on the general motor ability hypothesis, which states that motor competence is a one-dimensional trait underlying a wide range of motor skills. Yet, it is unclear whether the general motor ability hypothesis holds true in middle childhood, which is marked by an increased participation in sports and other types of physical activity. Therefore, the aim of the study was to evaluate the structure of motor competence in middle childhood using a test battery with a large item set.

Method: A cross-sectional design was used to collect motor competence data of 2538 children aged 6-11 years. Participants completed the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency-2nd Edition Short Form (BOT-2 SF), which consists of 14 skill items and covers different motor domains. In accordance with the BOT-2 SF manual, point scores were computed for each item. Polytomous Rasch analyses (ie, general partial credit model) were carried out to investigate the construct of motor competence.

Results: Rasch analyses revealed different items with unordered threshold parameters, due to ceiling effects. However, after empirically rescaling the category width for each item, follow-up analyses revealed a one-dimensional structure with 12 items.

Conclusion: The study provides some evidence of a one-dimensional construct (ie, motor competence) underlying motor assessment in middle childhood. Continued efforts should be made to ensure that valid composite scores are used in motor assessment and to better understand the development of motor competence across childhood and into adolescence and adulthood.

PubMed Disclaimer

References

REFERENCES

    1. Gallahue DL, Ozmun JC, Goodway JD. Understanding Motor Development: Infants, Children, Adolescents, Adults, 7th edn. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 2012.
    1. Haywood KM, Getchell N. Life Span Motor Development (6th edn). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 2014.
    1. Utesch T, Bardid F. Motor competence. In: Hackfort D, Schinke R, Strauss B, eds. Dictionary of Sport Psychology: Sport, Exercise, and Performing Arts. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2019:186.
    1. Robinson LE, Stodden DF, Barnett LM, et al. Motor competence and its effect on positive developmental trajectories of health. Sport Med. 2015;45(9):1273-1284.
    1. Barnett LM, Lai SK, Veldman SLC, et al. Correlates of gross motor competence in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sport Med. 2016;46(11):1663-1688.

Grants and funding