Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019 Oct 26;44(9):721-732.
doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjz064.

Regional Variation of Bitter Taste and Aftertaste in Humans

Affiliations

Regional Variation of Bitter Taste and Aftertaste in Humans

Molly J Higgins et al. Chem Senses. .

Abstract

Despite widespread and persistent myths of a tongue map, all 5 prototypical taste qualities are sensed over the entire tongue. However, modern psychophysical data also suggest there may be more nuanced differences in suprathreshold intensity across oral loci, especially for bitterness. Here, we test whether bitter stimuli matched for whole-mouth intensity differ in perceived intensity across regions of the oral cavity in 2 experiments. Experiment 1 consisted of a whole-mouth sip and spit approach and Experiment 2 consisted of a spatial taste test using cotton swabs. In Experiment 1, participants (n = 63) rated overall intensity of 3 bitter solutions at 5 different loci (front, middle, back of tongue; roof of mouth; and lip). Temporal effects were explored using in-mouth and aftertaste ratings. In Experiment 2, participants (n = 48) rated the intensity of quinine and Tetralone solutions after solutions were painted on fungiform, circumvallate, and foliate papillae with a swab. After the spatial taste test, participants completed a questionnaire on self-reported beer intake. Analysis of variance results of both experiments show a significant locus by stimulus interaction, suggesting different bitterants were perceived differently across the various loci. This result was apparently driven by low-intensity ratings for Tetralone on the anterior tongue. Aftertaste ratings in Experiment 1 also revealed significant temporal effects: ratings on the anterior tongue decreased for all bitterants and ratings for quinine decreased at all loci. Reasons for these effects are not known but may suggest differential expression of bitter taste receptors or differences in bitter agonist-receptor binding affinity across tongue regions.

Keywords: bitter taste; hops; papillae; spatial taste test; taste perception; tongue map.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Mean log intensity ratings and standard error of the mean for in-mouth ratings at different oral loci to represent the stimulus effect from Experiment 1. The intensities between stimuli at each locus were compared using t-tests (see text for details). To perform a Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons within a single locus, the P-value shown should be multiplied by 3 (e.g., P = 0.001 would be adjusted to P = 0.003).
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Mean log intensities and standard errors at different oral loci for aftertaste ratings made 15 s after spitting out the sample. Other details are the same as in Figure 1.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Mean in-mouth and aftertaste log intensity ratings for (a) quinine hydrochloride, (b) sucrose octaacetate (SOA), and (c) Tetralone, a hop extract, from Experiment 1. Comparisons at each locus represent t-tests following a significant locus by time interaction in the repeated measures ANOVA conducted separately for each stimulus (see text for details). To perform a Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons across the 5 different loci, the P-value shown should be multiplied by 5 (e.g., P = 0.005 would be adjusted to P = 0.025).
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Mean log intensity ratings from the spatial taste test used in Experiment 2. Significance for the t-tests between the solutions at each locus is noted. To perform a Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons across the 3 different loci, the P-value shown should be multiplied by 3 (e.g., P = 0.04 would be adjusted to P = 0.12).
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Intensity ratings (logged) for Tetralone on the foliate papillae plotted against self-reported pale ale consumption frequency (annualized, and quarter root transformed) for each participant in Experiment 2. The dashed line represents the correlation across all participants (n = 48), while the solid line represents the correlation among regular pale ale consumers (n = 28) after removing individuals who reported consumption of pale ales 1–2 times per year or less. These non-consumers (n = 20) are shown by the light gray shading in the bottom portion of the graph.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Allen AL, McGeary JE, Knopik VS, Hayes JE. 2013. Bitterness of the non-nutritive sweetener acesulfame potassium varies with polymorphisms in TAS2R9 and TAS2R31. Chem Senses. 38:379–389. - PMC - PubMed
    1. Appleton KM, Tuorila H, Bertenshaw EJ, de Graaf C, Mela DJ. 2018. Sweet taste exposure and the subsequent acceptance and preference for sweet taste in the diet: systematic review of the published literature. Am J Clin Nutr. 107:405–419. - PubMed
    1. Bartoshuk L. 1989. Clinical evaluation of the sense of taste. Ear Nose Throat J. 68:331–337. - PubMed
    1. Bartoshuk LM, Catalanotto F, Hoffman H, Logan H, Snyder DJ. 2012. Taste damage (otitis media, tonsillectomy and head and neck cancer), oral sensations and BMI. Physiol Behav. 107:516–526. - PubMed
    1. Bartoshuk LM, Duffy VB, Fast K, Green BG, Prutkin J, Snyder DJ. 2003. Labeled scales (e.g., category, Likert, VAS) and invalid across-group comparisons: what we have learned from genetic variation in taste. Food Qual Prefer. 14:125–138.

Publication types