Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2019 Aug 30:10:2030.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02030. eCollection 2019.

Disease Threat and the Functional Flexibility of Ingroup Derogation

Affiliations

Disease Threat and the Functional Flexibility of Ingroup Derogation

Qi Wu et al. Front Psychol. .

Abstract

While the findings from previous studies directly relate the ingroup derogation phenomenon to the evolved response of the behavioral immune system, there are three major limitations in the previous studies on the functional flexibility of ingroup derogation. The present study further investigated the functional flexibility of ingroup derogation by conducting three behavioral experiments on Chinese participants. In Experiment 1, we tested whether exposing to situational disease primes leads to an exaggerated ingroup derogation attitude by adopting a more rigorous control. In Experiment 2, we manipulated the source of disease threats to test whether the ingroup derogation mechanism adjusts its response according to the specific perceived vulnerabilities to the disease threats posed by ingroup and outgroup members. In Experiment 3, we tested whether recent illness promotes the expression of ingroup derogation attitudes. Results of the three experiments consistently showed that, the Chinese participants adjusted their ingroup derogation attitudes according to the external environmental disease cues (Experiments 1 and 2) and the internal physiological disease cues (Experiment 3). The results also showed that the ingroup derogation mechanism was sensitive to the specific perceived vulnerabilities to the ingroup disease threat and the outgroup disease threat (Experiment 2). Taken together, these results support the evolutionary hypothesis of ingroup derogation and suggest that the ingroup derogation found in East Asian cultures could be accounted by a functionally flexible disease-avoidance mechanism.

Keywords: behavioral immune system; disease threat; functional flexibility principle; ingroup derogation; smoke detection principle.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Degree of acceptance of faces labeled as ingroup members and outgroup members in Experiment 1. Error bars represent standard errors.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Degree of acceptance of faces labeled as ingroup members and outgroup members in Experiment 2. Error bars represent standard errors.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Degree of acceptance of faces labeled as ingroup members and outgroup members in Experiment 3. Error bars represent standard errors.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Aarøe L., Osmundsen M., Petersen M. B. (2016). Distrust as a disease avoidance strategy: individual differences in disgust sensitivity regulate generalized social trust. Front. Psychol. 7:1038. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01038, PMID: - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Allport G. (1958). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison Wesley.
    1. Ashburn-Nardo L., Knowles M., Monteith M. (2003). Black Americans’ implicit racial associations and their implications for intergroup judgment. Soc. Cogn. 21, 61–87. 10.1521/soco.21.1.61.21192 - DOI
    1. Axt J. R., Moran T., Bar-Anan Y. (2018). Simultaneous ingroup and outgroup favoritism in implicit social cognition. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 50, 268–284. 10.1177/0022022118814687 - DOI
    1. Barker J. L., Barclay P. (2016). Local competition increases people’s willingness to harm others. Evol. Hum. Behav. 37, 315–322. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.02.001, PMID: - DOI - PMC - PubMed

LinkOut - more resources